Understanding Syntax
eBook - ePub

Understanding Syntax

Maggie Tallerman

  1. 350 pages
  2. English
  3. ePUB (adapté aux mobiles)
  4. Disponible sur iOS et Android
eBook - ePub

Understanding Syntax

Maggie Tallerman

DĂ©tails du livre
Aperçu du livre
Table des matiĂšres
Citations

À propos de ce livre

Assuming no prior grammatical knowledge, Understanding Syntax explains and illustrates the major concepts, categories and terminology involved in the study of cross-linguistic syntax. Taking a theory-neutral and descriptive viewpoint throughout, this book:



  • introduces syntactic typology, syntactic description and the major typological categories found in the languages of the world;


  • clarifies with examples grammatical constructions and relationships between words in a clause, including word classes and their syntactic properties; grammatical relations such as subject and object; case and agreement processes; passives; questions and relative clauses;


  • features in-text and chapter-end exercises to extend the reader's knowledge of syntactic concepts and argumentation, drawing on data from over 100 languages;


  • highlights the principles involved in writing a brief syntactic sketch of language.

This fifth edition has been revised and updated to include extended exercises in all chapters, updated further readings, and more extensive checklists for students. Accompanying e-resources have also been updated to include hints for instructors and additional links to further reading.

Understanding Syntax is an essential textbook for students studying the description of language, cross-linguistic syntax, language typology and linguistic fieldwork.

Foire aux questions

Comment puis-je résilier mon abonnement ?
Il vous suffit de vous rendre dans la section compte dans paramĂštres et de cliquer sur « RĂ©silier l’abonnement ». C’est aussi simple que cela ! Une fois que vous aurez rĂ©siliĂ© votre abonnement, il restera actif pour le reste de la pĂ©riode pour laquelle vous avez payĂ©. DĂ©couvrez-en plus ici.
Puis-je / comment puis-je télécharger des livres ?
Pour le moment, tous nos livres en format ePub adaptĂ©s aux mobiles peuvent ĂȘtre tĂ©lĂ©chargĂ©s via l’application. La plupart de nos PDF sont Ă©galement disponibles en tĂ©lĂ©chargement et les autres seront tĂ©lĂ©chargeables trĂšs prochainement. DĂ©couvrez-en plus ici.
Quelle est la différence entre les formules tarifaires ?
Les deux abonnements vous donnent un accĂšs complet Ă  la bibliothĂšque et Ă  toutes les fonctionnalitĂ©s de Perlego. Les seules diffĂ©rences sont les tarifs ainsi que la pĂ©riode d’abonnement : avec l’abonnement annuel, vous Ă©conomiserez environ 30 % par rapport Ă  12 mois d’abonnement mensuel.
Qu’est-ce que Perlego ?
Nous sommes un service d’abonnement Ă  des ouvrages universitaires en ligne, oĂč vous pouvez accĂ©der Ă  toute une bibliothĂšque pour un prix infĂ©rieur Ă  celui d’un seul livre par mois. Avec plus d’un million de livres sur plus de 1 000 sujets, nous avons ce qu’il vous faut ! DĂ©couvrez-en plus ici.
Prenez-vous en charge la synthÚse vocale ?
Recherchez le symbole Écouter sur votre prochain livre pour voir si vous pouvez l’écouter. L’outil Écouter lit le texte Ă  haute voix pour vous, en surlignant le passage qui est en cours de lecture. Vous pouvez le mettre sur pause, l’accĂ©lĂ©rer ou le ralentir. DĂ©couvrez-en plus ici.
Est-ce que Understanding Syntax est un PDF/ePUB en ligne ?
Oui, vous pouvez accĂ©der Ă  Understanding Syntax par Maggie Tallerman en format PDF et/ou ePUB ainsi qu’à d’autres livres populaires dans Langues et linguistique et Syntaxe en linguistique. Nous disposons de plus d’un million d’ouvrages Ă  dĂ©couvrir dans notre catalogue.

Informations

Éditeur
Routledge
Année
2019
ISBN
9780429516740

1
What is syntax?

1.1 SOME CONCEPTS AND MISCONCEPTIONS

1.1.1 What is the study of syntax about?

This book is about the property of human language known as syntax. ‘Syntax’ means ‘sentence construction’: how words group together to make phrases and sentences. Some people also use the term GRAMMAR to mean the same as syntax, although most linguists follow the more recent practice whereby the grammar of a language includes all of its organizing principles: information about the sound system, about the form of words, how we adjust language according to context, and so on; syntax is only one part of this grammar.
The term ‘syntax’ is also used to mean the study of the syntactic properties of languages. In this sense it’s used in the same way as we use ‘stylistics’ to mean the study of literary style. We’re going to be studying how languages organize their syntax, so the scope of our study includes the classification of words, the order of words in phrases and sentences, the structure of phrases and sentences, and the different sentence constructions that languages use. We’ll be looking at examples of sentence structure from many different languages in this book, some related to English and others not. All languages have syntax, though that syntax may look radically different from that of English. My aim is to help you understand the way syntax works in languages, and to introduce the most important syntactic concepts and technical terms which you’ll need in order to see how syntax works in the world’s languages. We’ll encounter many grammatical terms, including ‘noun’, ‘verb’, ‘preposition’, ‘relative clause’, ‘subject’, ‘nominative’, ‘agreement’ and ‘passive’. I don’t expect you to know the meanings of any of these in advance. Oft en, terms are not formally defined when they are used for the first time, but they are illustrated so you can understand the concept, in preparation for a fuller discussion later on.
More complex terms and concepts (such as ‘case’ and ‘agreement’) are discussed more than once, and a picture of their meaning is built up over several chapters. A glossary at the end of the book provides definitions of important grammatical terms.
To help you understand what the study of syntax is about, we first need to discuss some things it isn’t about. When you read that ‘syntax’ is part of ‘grammar’, you may have certain impressions which differ from the aims of this book. So first, although we will be talking about grammar, this is not a DESCRIPTIVE GRAMMAR of English or any other language. Such books are certainly available, but they usually aim to catalogue the regularities and peculiarities of one language rather than looking at the organizing principles of language in general. Second, I won’t be trying to improve your ‘grammar’ of English. A PRESCRIPTIVE GRAMMAR (one that prescribes how the author thinks you should speak) might aim to teach you where to use who and whom; or when to say me and Ash and when to say Ash and I; it might tell you not to say different than or different to, or tell you to avoid split infinitives such as to boldly go. These things aren’t on our agenda, because they’re essentially a matter of taste – they are social, not linguistic matters.
In fact, as a linguist, my view is that if you’re a native speaker of English, no matter what your dialect, then you already know English grammar perfectly. And if you’re a native speaker of a different language, then you know the grammar of that language perfectly. By this, I don’t mean that you know (consciously) a few prescriptive rules, such as those mentioned in the last paragraph, but that you know (unconsciously) the much more impressive mental grammar of your own language – as do all its native speakers. Although we’ve all learnt this grammar, we can think of it as knowledge that we’ve never been taught, and it’s also knowledge that we can’t take out and examine. By the age of around 7, children have a fairly complete knowledge of the grammar of their native languages, and much of what happens aft er that age is learning more vocabulary. We can think of this as parallel to ‘learning’ how to walk. Children can’t be taught to walk; we all do it naturally when we’re ready, and we can’t say how we do it. Even if we come to understand exactly what muscle movements are required, and what brain circuitry is involved, we still don’t ‘know’ how we walk. Learning our native language is just the same: it happens without outside intervention and the resulting knowledge is inaccessible to us.
Here, you may object that you were taught the grammar of your native language. Perhaps you think that your parents set about teaching you it, or that you learnt it at school. But this is a misconception. All normally developing children in every culture learn their native language or languages to perfection without any formal teaching. Nothing more is required than the simple exposure to ordinary, live, human language within a society. To test whether this is true, we just need to ask if all cultures teach their children ‘grammar’. Since the answer is a resounding ‘no’, we can be sure that all children must be capable of constructing a mental grammar of their native languages without any formal instruction. Most linguists now believe that, in order to do this, human infants are born pre-programmed to learn language, in much the same way as we are pre-programmed to walk upright. All that’s needed for language to emerge is appropriate input data – hearing language (or seeing it; sign languages are full languages too) and taking part in interactions within the home and the wider society.
So if you weren’t taught the grammar of your native language, what was it you were being taught when your parents tried to get you not to say things like I ain’t done nowt wrong, or He’s more happier than what I am, or when your school teachers tried to stop you from using a preposition to end a sentence with? (Like the sentence I just wrote.) Again, consider learning to walk. Although children learn to do this perfectly without any parental instruction, their parents might not like the way the child slouches along, or scuffs the toes of their shoes on the ground. They may tell the child to stand up straight, or to stop wearing out their shoes. It’s not that the child’s way doesn’t function properly, it just doesn’t conform to someone’s idea of what is aesthetic, or classy. In just the same way, some people have the idea that certain forms of language are more beautiful, or classier, or are simply ‘correct’. But the belief that some forms of language are better than others has no linguistic basis. Since we oft en make social judgements about people based on their accent or dialect, we tend to transfer these judgements to their form of language. We may then think that some forms are undesirable, that some are ‘good’ and some ‘bad’. For a linguist, though, dialectal forms of a language don’t equate to ‘bad grammar’.
Again, you may object here that examples of NON-STANDARD English, such as those italicized in the last paragraph, or things like We done it well good, are sloppy speech, or perhaps illogical. This appeal to logic and precision makes prescriptive grammar seem to be on a higher plane than if it’s all down to social prejudice. So let’s examine the logic argument more closely, and see if it bears scrutiny. Many speakers of English are taught that ‘two negatives make a positive’, so that forms like (1) ‘really’ mean I did something wrong:
(1) I didn’t do nothing wrong.
Of course, this isn’t true. First, a speaker who uses a sentence like (1) doesn’t intend it to mean I did something wrong. Neither would any of their addressees, however much they despise the double negative, understand (1) to mean I did something wrong. Second, there are languages such as French and Breton which use a double negative as STANDARD, not a dialectal form, as (2) illustrates:1
fig0001
Example (2) shows that in Standard French the negative has two parts: in addition to the little negative word ne there’s another negative word jamais, ‘never’. Middle English (the English of roughly 1100 to 1500) also had a double negative. Ironically for the ‘logic’ argument, the variety of French that has the double negative is the most formal and prestigious variety, wherea...

Table des matiĂšres