Vision and Perception - A Conversation with Kalanit Grill-Spector
eBook - ePub

Vision and Perception - A Conversation with Kalanit Grill-Spector

Howard Burton

  1. English
  2. ePUB (apto para móviles)
  3. Disponible en iOS y Android
eBook - ePub

Vision and Perception - A Conversation with Kalanit Grill-Spector

Howard Burton

Detalles del libro
Vista previa del libro
Índice
Citas

Información del libro

This book is based on an in-depth conversation between Howard Burton and Kalanit Grill-Spector, Professor in Psychology and the Stanford Neurosciences Institute at Stanford University. Kalanit Grill-Spector is a vision specialist with a background in computational neuroscience. Her research examines how the brain processes visual information and perceives it. This extensive conversation explores how functional imaging techniques are used to visualize the brain in action and how it functions to recognize people, objects and places. Kalanit also discusses how the anatomical and functional properties of the brain change from infancy to childhood through adulthood, and how this development is related to improved visual recognition abilities.Further topics include Kalanit Grill-Spector's discovery of a particular face-selective region in the brain, her groundbreaking research related to the neural processing of this particular region and the fascinating experiments that she has been involved with that suggest that there is indeed a strong causal link between that region and our facial recognition perception.This carefully-edited book includes an introduction, Facing Facts, and questions for discussion at the end of each chapter: I. Neuroimaging - A transformative technologyII. Discovering Her Passion - A glimpse of the joy of vision III. Vision Unveiled - Our current understandingIV. Experimental Evidence - Many discoveries; even more to doV. A Startling Result - Stumbling upon specialized hardwareVI. Neuroplasticity - Assessing flexibilityVII. The Road Ahead - Better measurements, better models, deeper understandingAbout Ideas Roadshow Conversations Series: Presented in an accessible, conversational format, Ideas Roadshow books not only explore frontline academic research featuring world-leading researchers but also reveal the inspirations and personal journeys behind the research.

Preguntas frecuentes

¿Cómo cancelo mi suscripción?
Simplemente, dirígete a la sección ajustes de la cuenta y haz clic en «Cancelar suscripción». Así de sencillo. Después de cancelar tu suscripción, esta permanecerá activa el tiempo restante que hayas pagado. Obtén más información aquí.
¿Cómo descargo los libros?
Por el momento, todos nuestros libros ePub adaptables a dispositivos móviles se pueden descargar a través de la aplicación. La mayor parte de nuestros PDF también se puede descargar y ya estamos trabajando para que el resto también sea descargable. Obtén más información aquí.
¿En qué se diferencian los planes de precios?
Ambos planes te permiten acceder por completo a la biblioteca y a todas las funciones de Perlego. Las únicas diferencias son el precio y el período de suscripción: con el plan anual ahorrarás en torno a un 30 % en comparación con 12 meses de un plan mensual.
¿Qué es Perlego?
Somos un servicio de suscripción de libros de texto en línea que te permite acceder a toda una biblioteca en línea por menos de lo que cuesta un libro al mes. Con más de un millón de libros sobre más de 1000 categorías, ¡tenemos todo lo que necesitas! Obtén más información aquí.
¿Perlego ofrece la función de texto a voz?
Busca el símbolo de lectura en voz alta en tu próximo libro para ver si puedes escucharlo. La herramienta de lectura en voz alta lee el texto en voz alta por ti, resaltando el texto a medida que se lee. Puedes pausarla, acelerarla y ralentizarla. Obtén más información aquí.
¿Es Vision and Perception - A Conversation with Kalanit Grill-Spector un PDF/ePUB en línea?
Sí, puedes acceder a Vision and Perception - A Conversation with Kalanit Grill-Spector de Howard Burton en formato PDF o ePUB, así como a otros libros populares de Ciencias biológicas y Neurociencia. Tenemos más de un millón de libros disponibles en nuestro catálogo para que explores.

Información

Año
2021
ISBN
9781771700573

The Conversation

Photo of Kalanit Grill-Spectori and Howard Burton in conversation

I. Neuroimaging

A transformative technology

HB: Since you kindly gave me a tour of your fMRI facilities here at Stanford, let me just dive in and ask some questions about that. How does functional MRI differ from the normal MRI machine that most people are familiar with—for medical injuries and so forth—and how does it differ from PET scans which people also might have heard of?
KGS: fMRI has really been a revolution in cognitive neuroscience. It started in the early 90s. It’s the same machine that does the MRI for a knee scan, for example. The only thing is that, instead of measuring the tissue, we’re measuring changes in brain metabolism.
When you use your brain, to do some sensory processing for example, your brain uses oxygen, and changes in oxygenation levels affect the local magnetic field; this is what’s picked up by the scanner. The patient is placed inside the scanner, his or her head is placed in a coil, and this coil picks up signals in the brain that are linked to neural activation in the regions that are activated by whatever task the person is doing.
HB: When I first heard about fMRI I was confused about that because I thought, Oh, they’re measuring the brain, so they must be measuring the electrical signals. Of course what they’re doing is measuring, just as you said, the oxygen related to the blood supply that’s flowing into specific brain areas because of neural activity.
KGS: Yes. We’re not measuring direct neural activity. We’re measuring a BOLD signal, a blood-oxygen-level-dependent signal. In fact, you might think there would be less oxygen because you’ve used it for the metabolism, but the brain overcompensates so you get an overflow of oxygenated haemoglobin. Really what we are picking up with a scanner is the amount of deoxygenated haemoglobin, and that actually gets washed off; this is why the signal goes up. So it’s an indirect measure of brain activity.
The reason that it’s different from PET—positron emission tomography—is that it’s non-invasive. For PET you need to inject subjects with a radioactive material. That’s an invasive procedure. With fMRI you don’t inject anything. This has really been the power of fMRI technology because you can study the same person and run the experiments over and over again, or over time, or over development, or over the lifespan. This has been a really big breakthrough because you can peer into people’s brains without doing anything to them invasively.
HB: And this was developed in the early 90s?
KGS: The first fMRI papers were published in Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the USA in 1992. There were two groups that did this in parallel. One was a group at Bell Labs led by Seiji Ogawa. The other group was at Massachusetts General Hospital and the first author on that paper was Ken Kwong. Basically they conducted the first experiments where they showed people pictures versus no pictures—they had flashing checkerboards—and they could see an increase in the back of the brain where the visual cortex is located when people saw stuff versus when they didn’t.
HB: So in your own studies, subjects go into this machine and you have them focused on doing particular tasks and thinking about particular things, or seeing particular things. How long do they stay in there for, in general?
KGS: They usually stay between an hour and two. Usually what we’ll do is put the subject in the scanner and first run a brain anatomy scan. The reason we want to see their brain anatomy is because we are interested in which part of their brain is involved in what function. This allows us to create—I’ll show you later—these beautiful cortical reconstructions in which we can see the brain from all three dimensions, because the way we acquire information in the scanner is in slices, like in a CT scan, so we can get the 3D reconstruction. That takes about five to ten minutes. That’s called just MRI, anatomical MRI.
HB: Let me just stop you there for a moment. Does this mean that people’s brain anatomy differs significantly? I would have thought that this would be relatively constant. But it’s not?
KGS: There are two things to consider here. The first is that, as a field, we are interested in how brain anatomy does change. I, for example, am looking at how it might change from childhood to adulthood. There are some things that will happen as a result of certain diseases, like Alzheimer’s disease for example, where there are actually changes to the brain anatomy because of the disease.
The second point is that we want to look really closely at how function is implemented in each person’s brain. So your brain and my brain have the same general pattern of what we call cortical folding—there are hills and valleys—but there are idiosyncrasies for each brain and we really want to understand the relationship between function and anatomy in each person’s brain. So we want to take a detailed picture of every subject’s brain.
HB: So there is really significant variation between different people? That’s fascinating. I never would have thought that.
KGS: On one hand there is variety; on the other hand there is stability. One of the things we’re trying to figure out is what is stable and what is variable across people. There is more variety than, for example, the hand; there are always five fingers on your hand. There is a little bit more variety in the number of cortical folds, but the big ones are very stable across people.
HB: Right—but I had interrupted you. You were telling me that these anatomical scans were the first things that you do.
KGS: That’s the first thing we do and that takes between five to ten minutes. Sometimes we do another kind of scan called diffusion tensor imaging or diffusion weighted imaging. That lets us measure how water diffuses in the brain. We do this to look at the wiring of the brain—which part of the brain is connected to another part of the brain. There are these really big white matter bundles; they’re called fascicles. Because they are myelinated, they are very directional, so the water doesn’t diffuse in all directions.
HB: Hold on a sec. Where is the water in my brain?
KGS: Your brain is all water.
HB: That’s not just my brain presumably?
KGS: Actually, all we are measuring in fMRI is how the magnetic field affects the water molecules. We’re measuring hydrogen atoms basically. Because the water doesn’t propagate freely, most of the direction of diffusion will be parallel to the fascicle, and we can measure the connectivity, the white matter connection, between one part of the brain and another part of the brain. This is another ty...

Índice

  1. A Note on the Text
  2. Introduction
  3. The Conversation
  4. Continuing the Conversation
Estilos de citas para Vision and Perception - A Conversation with Kalanit Grill-Spector

APA 6 Citation

Burton, H. (2021). Vision and Perception - A Conversation with Kalanit Grill-Spector ([edition unavailable]). Open Agenda Publishing inc. Retrieved from https://www.perlego.com/book/2699831/vision-and-perception-a-conversation-with-kalanit-grillspector-pdf (Original work published 2021)

Chicago Citation

Burton, Howard. (2021) 2021. Vision and Perception - A Conversation with Kalanit Grill-Spector. [Edition unavailable]. Open Agenda Publishing inc. https://www.perlego.com/book/2699831/vision-and-perception-a-conversation-with-kalanit-grillspector-pdf.

Harvard Citation

Burton, H. (2021) Vision and Perception - A Conversation with Kalanit Grill-Spector. [edition unavailable]. Open Agenda Publishing inc. Available at: https://www.perlego.com/book/2699831/vision-and-perception-a-conversation-with-kalanit-grillspector-pdf (Accessed: 15 October 2022).

MLA 7 Citation

Burton, Howard. Vision and Perception - A Conversation with Kalanit Grill-Spector. [edition unavailable]. Open Agenda Publishing inc., 2021. Web. 15 Oct. 2022.