Social Research Methods by Example
eBook - ePub

Social Research Methods by Example

Applications in the Modern World

Yasemin Besen-Cassino, Dan Cassino

Share book
  1. 420 pages
  2. English
  3. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  4. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Social Research Methods by Example

Applications in the Modern World

Yasemin Besen-Cassino, Dan Cassino

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

Social Research Methods by Example shows students how researchers carry out work on the cutting edge of social science. The authors illustrate every point through engaging, thought-provoking examples from real research. The language is jargon-free, making research methods less intimidating and more relatable. The text is divided into three major sections, the first of which introduces students to the principles of research through examples from various fields. The second section walks students through the major types of social science research, with each chapter focusing on a different technique. The third section shows students how to carry out basic quantitative data analysis in SPSS. The final chapter shows how technological advances have changed the way researchers are working, and looks at the direction of social science research in the future.

Social Research Methods by Example not only introduces students to the principles of social science research, but gives them a toolbox to carry out their own. By the time they are finished with the book, students will be conversant with many of the most important studies in the history of social science. They will understand not only how to conduct research, but also how the field has evolved over time.

Frequently asked questions

How do I cancel my subscription?
Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on ā€œCancel Subscriptionā€ - itā€™s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time youā€™ve paid for. Learn more here.
Can/how do I download books?
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
What is the difference between the pricing plans?
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlegoā€™s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan youā€™ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
What is Perlego?
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, weā€™ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Do you support text-to-speech?
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Is Social Research Methods by Example an online PDF/ePUB?
Yes, you can access Social Research Methods by Example by Yasemin Besen-Cassino, Dan Cassino in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Social Sciences & Sociology. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Publisher
Routledge
Year
2017
ISBN
9781351816656
Edition
1

CHAPTER 1
Introduction to Research Methods

Learning Objectives

ā€¢ Be able to explain the interaction between the individual and the environment
ā€¢ Be able to identify variables
ā€¢ Be able to identify components of the wheel of science and describe the processes of the scientific method
ā€¢ Be able to describe the concept of causality
ā€¢ Be able to identify different levels of measurement

EFFECTS OF SOCIETY: WHY OUR ENVIRONMENT MATTERS!

Jonah Berger, Marc Meredith, and S. Christian Wheelerā€”Polling Places and Vote Choice (2008)

A common criticism of social science research is that itā€™s not really necessary. We donā€™t need scientists to figure out why people do what they doā€”if we want to know, we can just ask people. Everyone believes that they have reasons for their behavior, and that they know what those reasons are, but social science allows researchers to show that while people may have reasons for their actions and beliefs, they donā€™t always know what those reasons are. The phenomena that social scientists study donā€™t exist in a vacuum; theyā€™re embedded in a social structure, in a physical place, and among other people, and this context has enormous effects on peopleā€™s behavior. The fact that these effects are often invisible to the individuals theyā€™re affecting doesnā€™t make them any less real.
Berger, Meredith, and Wheeler (2008) examined how people voted in a 2000 Arizona ballot initiative. The initiativeā€”called Proposition 301ā€”asked voters to increase the state sales tax from 5 percent to 5.6 percent, in order to fund an increase in education spending. Now, there are many factors that might lead voters to support or oppose this measure: whether theyā€™re Republicans or Democrats, their attitudes toward education, their income, their gender, whether or not they have school-age children. Researchers often ask voters coming out of polling places why they voted the way they did, and voters typically say things like, ā€œI care about education,ā€ or ā€œI donā€™t want higher taxes.ā€ Almost none of these voters, however, will say that they were influenced by where they voted. While individual votes are secret, the researchers were able to look at how people voted on Proposition 301 in each of 2,027 polling places and see how the results differed based on the actual polling place.
Some of the polling places were government buildings or community centers; others were churches or schools. Berger, Meredith, and Wheeler (2008) found that the location of the polling place had a significant effect on how people voted on the proposition: when people voted in a school, they were about 2 points more likely to support raising taxes to fund education (55 percent versus 53 percent). The results show that there are a significant number of voters who apparently cast their ballot the way that they did because they were voting in a schoolā€”but itā€™s unlikely that many of these voters realized this. They thought that they were voting a certain way for their own reasons, but carefully constructed social science research can tell us what was really happening.
This book explores the ways in which social scientists explore the effects of the social environment on individual and group behaviors. It is a handbook for social scientists, not just outlining how social scientists study phenomena, but also describing some of the most important studies in the last 100 years of social science research. When done right, social science can understand people better than they understand themselves, and the job of this book is to teach readers how to do it right.

THINKING THROUGH VARIABLES: HOW RESEARCHERS WORK

Brian Wansinkā€”Mindless Eating (2006)

Overeating has moved from an individual problem to a major social problem, with increasing numbers of Americans being classified as overweight or obese, and government healthcare programs being stretched by the added expense. To understand how to get people to eat less, itā€™s necessary to understand why people eat moreā€”and this is why Brian Wansink, a professor of consumer behavior and nutrition science, founded Cornellā€™s Food and Brand Lab. There are all sorts of potential explanations for why people overeatā€”the taste of the food, the smell, hungerā€”and the job of social science research is to isolate these potential causes and determine which ones really matter.
Wansink (2006) argues that the cause of most overeating is the availability of food: People eat more simply because thereā€™s more around. To test this, he designed a series of studies in which he looked at all of the potential causes for why people eat too much. In one of these studies, people in Chicago were given tickets to see the afternoon matinee of a new action movie. When they showed up, the people who were part of the studyā€”a group social scientists refer to as participants, a term which has replaced the older subjectsā€”were given a surprise: a free bucket of popcorn. The movie was scheduled to start at 1:05 in the afternoon, right after lunch, so many of the participants who arrived at the movie theater had just eaten. The goal here was to try and make sure that the participants werenā€™t eating the popcorn just because they were hungry.
Once they came in, the participants were randomly given either a medium-sized bag of popcorn or an extra-large bucket, which Wansink describes as being ā€œbigger than their heads.ā€ The researchers believed that the participants who were given the extra-large bucket would eat more, just because thereā€™s more available, but thereā€™s a problem. What if the participants eat more just because the popcorn tastes good? In addition to controlling for the hunger of the participants, the researchers also have to control for the taste of the popcorn.
All of these factorsā€”how hungry the participants are, the size of the popcorn theyā€™re given, how good the popcorn tastesā€”are all variables. The key aspect of variables is that they vary within the study, taking on different values, as opposed to constants, which remain the same. Within society, age is a variableā€”some people are older or younger than othersā€”so researchers can examine the effect of age. If something is not a variable in the study, itā€™s a constant, and researchers arenā€™t able to look at any effects it may have. For instance, if all of the participants in a study are men or college students, then thereā€™s no way for researchers to determine how being a man or being a college student affects whatever the study is looking at.
The variable that a social scientist is interested in explaining is referred to as a dependent variable. The dependent variable is the outcome; the researcher believes that it is caused by the other variables in the model. These variables, the ones that cause the dependent variable, are the independent variables. One of the first steps in any social science study is to think through these variables, and a big part of being a social scientist is seeing the world in terms of independent and dependent variables, cause and effect.
fig1_4_1.webp
In this case, the dependent variable is how much the participants eat, measured by the amount of popcorn eating and the number of calories consumed. While there is only one dependent variable, there can be multiple independent variables. In this case, the independent variables include the amount of popcorn thatā€™s available and how hungry the participants are.

Problem of Spuriousness

Before Wansink 2010 can be sure that availability leads his participants to overeat, he has to control for other variables, like the taste of the popcorn. What if the reason why people overeat is not simply because the popcorn is there, but rather because they enjoy the taste? Good popcorn might be so warm and buttery that the participants just canā€™t resist it. Participants who get the larger buckets will eat more because itā€™s so good. This would be a problem of spuriousness. Spuriousness refers to the unwanted effects of lurking variables (also called confounds, especially in experimental studies), independent variables that arenā€™t accounted for but have an impact on the dependent variable regardless. If thereā€™s spuriousness, it might look like an independent variable is causing the dependent variable when, in fact, an unmeasured third variable is driving the relationship.
fig1_4_2.webp
To eliminate this potential problem, Wansink (2006) and his team made sure that no one was eating the popcorn because they liked the taste. Rather than give the participants warm, buttery popcorn, the researchers gave them cold popcorn that had been popped five days beforehand and stored in sterile containers until it squeaked like plastic and was described as being like ā€œeating Styrofoam packing peanutsā€ by one of the participants. At the end of the movie, the participants were given a survey asking them about how much they had consumed. When asked, ā€œDo you think you consumed more because you had the larger size?ā€ they generally said no. The responses were things like ā€œThat wouldnā€™t happen to me,ā€ ā€œThings like that donā€™t trick me,ā€ or ā€œIā€™m pretty good at knowing when Iā€™m full.ā€
In reality, though, participants who were given the larger buckets of squeaky popcorn ate more. After weighing the buckets, researchers concluded that the participants who were randomly assigned to get the larger buckets consumed 173 more calories, approximately equal to 21 more dips into the bucket. This is 53 percent more than those given the medium-size buckets. Based on these findings, it seems likely that the participants were eating more popcorn not because they were hungry, or because it tasted good, but simply because it was there. Again, the participants donā€™t say that theyā€™re eating more just because thereā€™s more thereā€”Wansink would argue that theyā€™re not thinking about why they eat at allā€”but a carefully designed study can allow researchers to know more about why people do things than the people themselves know.
Still, this is just one study, and one of the hallmarks of social science is whatā€™s called triangulation, or measuring the same theory in different ways in order to be more certain about the findings. In addition to availability, Wansink (2006) argues that people rely on social cues in order to decide how much to eat. One of Wansinkā€™s labs also doubles as a fine dining restaurant twice a month, offering an all-inclusive, prix-fixe dinner for less than $25. In all respects, it looks like a high-end fine dining restaurant, with the big difference being that the diners are actually participants in experiments. In 2004, diners on one night got a special treat: a free Cabernet Sauvignon. It was not a special wine, though: it was a $2 bottle of Charles Shaw from Trader Joes (known as Two Buck Chuck). However, the labels of the bottles were changed. Diners on the left side of the restaurant were offered the same wine with a label for Noahā€™s Winery, located in California. The diners on the right side got an identical label for Noahā€™s Winery, except the location was listed as North Dakota.
Even though they were given the same wine, diners who were given wine with North Dakota labels had lower expectations and rated the wine as tasting bad and their food as inferior in taste and quality. Diners who were given wine with the California label ate 11 percent more of their food and lingered 10 minutes more at their tables after dinner. Even though the wine and the food provided were identical, the label on the bottle made the diners on the left think of the meal as a special occasion. They ate more without any idea about what was causing them to do so.
Social scientists also have to be careful about the trap of monocausation. While natural scientists often deal with phenomena that have one cause (increased pressure leads to increased heat, for instance), the phenomena that social scientists are most interested in have multiple and overlapping causes. Sorting out all of these causes is made even more difficult by the fact that people are embedded in a social context, and itā€™s necessary to understand not just how individual-level causes are affecting the individual, but also how the other people around that individual are influencing his or her behavior.
It may seem that Wasink has overeating all figured out, but there are almost always more causesā€”more independent variablesā€”that could be important. For instance, psychology professor John DeCastro (1990) shows that even the presence of others makes us eat more: Diners eat 35 percent more if they eat with one other person than they would if they dined alone. If people eat with a group of seven or more, they consume 96 percent more food than they would consume alone! Of course, all of these factors may matter more, or less, depending on the availability of food and the social context of the meal, both of which increase the amount that people eat. While thereā€™s one main dependent variable in these studiesā€”the amount that people eatā€”there are generally many independent variables, enough that the search for more causes is never really over.
fig1_6_1.webp
In all of the above examples, the unit of analysis was the individual: why one person, rather than another, chooses to overeat. H...

Table of contents