Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals
eBook - ePub

Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals

  1. English
  2. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  3. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

Environmental enrichment is a simple and effective means of improving animal welfare in any species – companion, farm, laboratory and zoo. For many years, it has been a popular area of research, and has attracted the attention and concerns of animal keepers and carers, animal industry professionals, academics, students and pet owners all over the world.

This book is the first to integrate scientific knowledge and principles to show how environmental enrichment can be used on different types of animal. Filling a major gap, it considers the history of animal keeping, legal issues and ethics, right through to a detailed exploration of whether environmental enrichment actually works, the methods involved, and how to design and manage programmes.

  • The first book in a major new animal welfare series
  • Draws together a large amount of research on different animals
  • Provides detailed examples and case studies
  • An invaluable reference tool for all those who work with or study animals in captivity

This book is part of the UFAW/Wiley-Blackwell Animal Welfare Book Series. This major series of books produced in collaboration between UFAW (The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare), and Wiley-Blackwell provides an authoritative source of information on worldwide developments, current thinking and best practice in the field of animal welfare science and technology. For details of all of the titles in the series see www.wiley.com/go/ufaw.

Frequently asked questions

Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Yes, you can access Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals by Robert J. Young in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Medicina & Medicina veterinaria. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Year
2013
ISBN
9781118699553
Edition
1

1

Environmental Enrichment: an Historical Perspective

In 1985, the Congress of the USA passed amendments to the Animal Welfare Act that directed the Animal Plant and Health Inspection Service (APHIS) to promulgate regulations that provide for the psychological well-being of non-human primates (Bloomsmith et al., 1991). In February 1991, the US Drug Administration/APHIS issued a final ruling that states: ‘Dealers, exhibitors, and research facilities must develop, document and follow an appropriate plan for environment enhancement adequate to promote the psychological well-being of non-human primates’.
In the UK, while environmental enrichment is not a legal requirement in animal keeping institutions (i.e., farms, laboratories and zoos), it certainly helps to justify laboratory animal experiments (see Chapter 7) and in the UK, zoo visitors expect to see it being implemented (Reade & Waran, 1996). Personally, I have run workshops and courses on this subject from countries as diverse as Brazil and Russia. Television programmes about animals in the UK often feature stories about how to enrich the lives of pet species (see Chapters 7 and 13). How did we arrive at this heightened level of interest in environmental enrichment? A historical perspective is very useful on any subject matter, since knowing where we have come from often determines where we should go. However, before starting we need to define what we mean by environmental enrichment.

1.1 Definitions

‘Environmental enrichment is a concept which describes how the environments of captive animals can be changed for the benefit of the inhabitants. Behavioural opportunities that may arise or increase as a result of environmental enrichment can be appropriately described as behavioural enrichment’ (Shepherdson, 1994).
Alternatively, environmental enrichment is ‘a process for improving or enhancing zoo animal environments and care within the context of their inhabitants’ behavioral biology and natural history. It is a dynamic process in which changes to structures and husbandry practices are made with the goal of increasing behavioral choices to animals and drawing out their species appropriate behaviors and abilities, thus enhancing animal welfare’. (BHAG, 1999, provided by Valerie Hare).

1.1.1 Goals

In terms of practically implementing environmental enrichment it is easier to think of its goals rather than the various definitions that exist (see above). The goals are to:
(1) increase behavioural diversity;
(2) reduce the frequencies of abnormal behaviour;
(3) increase the range or number of normal (i.e. wild) behaviour patterns;
(4) increase positive utilisation of the environment;
(5) increase the ability to cope with challenges in a more normal way.
(Modified after Shepherdson, 1989; Chamove & Moodie, 1990)

1.1.2 Types of enrichment

Environmental enrichment is a term that applies to heterogeneous methods of improving animal welfare that includes everything from social companionship to toys. Bloomsmith et al. (1991) identified five major types of enrichment, each of which can be subdivided:
(1) Social
(1.1) Contact
(1.1.1) Conspecific (pair, group, temporary, permanent)
(1.1.2) Contraspecific (human, non-human)
(1.2) Non-contact
(1.2.1) (visual, auditory, co-operative device)
(1.2.2) (human, non-human)
(2) Occupational
(2.1) Psychological (puzzles, control of environment)
(2.2) Exercise (mechanical devices, run)
(3) Physical
(3.1) Enclosure
(3.1.1) Size (alteration)
(3.1.2) Complexity (panels for apparatus)
(3.2) Accessories
(3.2.1) Internal
(3.2.1.1) Permanent (furniture, bars)
(3.2.1.2) Temporary (toys, ropes, substrates)
(3.2.2) External (hanging objects, puzzles)
(4) Sensory
(4.1) Visual (tapes, television, images, windows)
(4.2) Auditory (music, vocalisations)
(4.3) Other stimuli (olfactory, tactile, taste)
(5) Nutritional
(5.1) Delivery (frequency, schedule, presentation, processing)
(5.2) Type (novel, variety, browse, treats)
In Chapters 8–11 I discuss all the different types of enrichment and strategies for implementing them for any species of animal held in captivity. The origins of animal keeping, animal welfare and environmental enrichment are pertinent to the types of enrichment we might use and, therefore, these subjects are discussed in the remainder of this chapter.

1.2 A Short History of Animal Keeping

The origins of zoos have been extremely well documented by Bostock (1993) in his book Animal Rights and Zoos. To summarise briefly, the first major collections of exotic animals were housed by the ancient Egyptians (around 3000 bc). These collections were maintained for two broad reasons: (1) many of the species kept had religious significance; (2) the possession of exotic animals was regarded as a status symbol. The use of animals as status symbols by rich and royal families across Europe and the Middle East continued until around 1800. In London, the Tower of London housed the royal family’s collection of exotic animals, which had included lions and polar bears (which were often presented as gifts). Then, in the early 1800s, scientists such as Darwin started to take a serious scientific interest in the Animal Kingdom, especially in classifying animals into related groups (i.e., systematics). To facilitate their work these scientists needed large collections of different species and ones that could be easily observed (this meant small barren enclosures). It was at this time in London that the royal animal collection was moved from The Tower to Regent’s Park. Sir Stamford Raffles founded London Zoo in Regent’s Park in 1826. For the first twenty years of its life the zoo was only open to bona fide scientists before finally allowing entrance to the fee paying public. Soon after the public was given access to London Zoo, letters of complaint and criticisms of the high death rates of the animals started to appear in The Times newspaper. The animals were largely dying from physical health problems, such as disease. The zoo responded to the problems by increasing levels of hygiene and ensuring that all newly built enclosures could be easily cleaned (this meant hard surfaced, small barren enclosures – now referred to as hard architecture) – conditions that still exist in many zoos today despite advances in veterinary medicine and despite the work of Hagenbeck on the design of naturalistic enclosures (see below).
Unfortunately for zoo animals, zoo architecture often followed trends in human architecture. In the UK in the 1960s functionalism and constructions of reinforced concrete were in fashion for human architecture. Thus, architects such as Berthold Lubetkin were designing both high-rise flats for humans and zoo-animal enclosures (much of his work can still be seen in Dudley Zoo, UK). It was not until the 1960s with the growing interest in animal welfare (spurred on by Ruth Harrison’s (1964) book Animal Machines, see below) and the recognition of the need for conserving species from extinction by captive breeding, that many zoos developed more animal-welfare-friendly enclosures. This is despite the fact that some zoos had for many years recognised the potential for animal suffering. The archives of Edinburgh Zoo contained copies of all the annual reports produced from 1909 (before the zoo opened) to the present day. These reports make interesting reading; I have picked out below some relevant extracts to demonstrate the evolution of zoos:
1911 A paper was presented to the zoological society which suggested that if the zoo acquired polar bears it would have to provide toys and other objects as outlets for this species’ well-known playful and exploratory behaviour.
1930s The zoological society discussed the building of a tiger enclosure with an undulating front to prevent the tigers from performing their well-known parading up and down behaviour.
1950s The zoo received criticisms in newspapers for overcrowding in the bear enclosures.
1960s The language in the annual reports became more scientific and the animals were no longer referred to as ‘the inmates’. At the same time, animals were no longer referred to by their given names.
1973 The first environmental enrichment study was conducted in the zoo by a student (Charles Watson) from the University of Edinburgh.
1981 The chimpanzees were group-housed in a large enclosure with an artificial termite mound.
1990s Many studies on behaviour and environmental enrichment were reported as being conducted within the zoo.
It is sobering to reflect on some of the significance of these extracts, particularly that for 1911 and the fact that most zoos did little about polar bear enrichment until the 1980s (Ames, 1993). The 1930s report is clearly an unconscious reference to stereotypic route pacing, which clearly was unpopular with the visitors or why else would the zoo seek to eliminate it. A study by Lyons et al. (1997) has shown that this enclosure is successful at preventing the expression of pacing behaviour but this does not mean an improvement in animal welfare (see Chapter 3). The first observations of stereotypic behaviour in zoo animals were made at this time in Germany (Meyer-Holzapfel, 1968). The reasons why ideas or information that could improve animal welfare took so long to implement are unclear. (I speculate that it probably relates to the greater public awareness of animal welfare in the 1960s, and some people have suggested that the proliferation of wildlife documentaries at this time caused a change in public attitudes. It is ironic, however, that many wildlife documentaries use zoo animals for their close-ups or when they wish for a visually spectacular behaviour pattern.)
The present trends in zoo enclosure designs in western countries tend to reflect the roles of the modern zoo, in conservation, education, research and recreation (Kreger et al., 1998). For example, in the US and Europe naturalistic enclosure designs are now popular because they facilitate environmental education programmes, i.e. they place the animal in the context of its environment. Today, the conservation work of zoos is co-ordinated by national (e.g. American Zoo and Aquarium Association) and international organisations (e.g. World Zoo Organisation). The main challenge facing zoos today is to house animals in enclosures that, as Tudge (1992) put it, conserves the whole animal (i.e. behaviour as well as genes). Environmental enrichment has a significant role to play with respect to this.
Humans (Homo sapiens) and human ancestors (e.g. H. habilis, H. erectus and Neanderthals) have been exploiting animals for food for at least two million years. Animals were principally exploited by hunting until relatively recent times (16000 years ago) when some modern humans desisted from their nomadic hunter-gatherer lifestyle and commenced farming in one location (Passariello, 1999). The next significant advances were made when the first animals were domesticated, since domesticated animals are much easier to manage. Domestication is basically a process whereby a species becomes adapted to living with and being managed by humans. This undoubtedly involved the selection of various behavioural, physiological and morphological traits. A key trait would be reduced fear of humans. Such traits that arose during early domestications are likely to be the by-product of the process (i.e. those sheep with less fear of humans produced the most off-spring) rather than a deliberate selection policy by ancient farmers. The domestic sheep was the first food animal to be domesticated (from the Asiatic mouflon) around 9000 years ago in the Middle East. Once humans had a species ‘tamed’ in captivity they could then start deliberate selection for desirable characteristics, such a fast growth rate and large body size. There is evidence that sheep were being selected for particular coat characteristics 8000 years ago (Pond, 1994). The world population was five million people at the time farming of animals commenced. 8000 years later it was 500 million and during the last millennium it increased to more than five billion people, having tripled between 1900 and 2000. Over this long period of time agricultural practices gradually evolved and became more refined, and species were continuously selected for traits useful to humans, e.g. increased litter size in pigs (Pond, 1994). The next major change in agricultural practices came after 1945. During the Second World War (1939–45) the UK discovered it needed to import food from the US as it was not self-sufficient in food production. After the war politicians regarded self-sufficiency in food production as essential to national security and encouraged farmers to find methods of producing more food but on the same amount of land. This gave rise to intensive systems of animal husbandry, which have been heavily criticised for their animal welfare standards (e.g. Harrison, 1964). Food from intensive farming systems was popular with the general public because it was cheap to buy. Much of the farm animal husbandry and enclosures we have today are the result of this pressure to be self-sufficient in food. Of course, public concern has created some changes, for example, the UK ban on keeping pregnant pigs in small metal crates (tethered to the crate by a short chain) and the ban on battery-cage egg production in Switzerland. However, alternative production methods produce smaller profits (Bennett, 1997) and often a premium priced product. In the UK, the Royal Society for the Protection of Animals (RSPCA) endorses high-welfare farms with the ‘Freedom Food’ label allowing farmers to sell their product at a premium (Kells et al., 2001).
The first animal to be domesticated was the domestic dog, from the Asiatic wolf, around 12000 years ago in the Middle East. The process of domestication probably started with some wolves approaching close to human settlements and being fed. Humans quickly realised that wolves could prove to be useful ‘look-outs’ and had the potential to help with hunting animals. Over a period of time the wild wolves became tamed and the process of domestication began. There is archaeological evidence that different breeds of dogs existed 10 000 years ago. Pet breeds of dogs almost certainly were bred from dogs kept as working animals, i.e. dogs were domesticated to work for humans and then became pets – they were not domesticated to be pets (Passariello, 1999). The ancient Egyptian pharaohs kept several breeds of dogs as long ago as 1900 bc. The Chinese emperors had the pekinese breed created for them at least one thousand years ago. There now exist more than 400 breeds of dog. Over the course of the human–dog history, the environment of the dog in western countries has become much more restrictive, i.e. most dogs are restricted to their owners’ house except during exercise. However, it would be wrong to think of pet-keeping as a western-society tradition: explorers discovering and charting North and South America in the 1600s and 1700s found pet-keeping to be common among indigenous peoples. The number of exotic species being kept as pets in Western societies has been rising steadily since the 1960s. Many of these species, such as reptiles, have highly specific housing and husbandry requirements to experience a good level of animal welfare. Pets in general are the forgotten animals of public concern in animal welfare (see Chapter 7) and may experience a low level of well-being, especially psychological.
Science only started to become a major force in changing human lifestyles during the period of the Industrial Revolution (1820s onwards). It was only really with the drive to develop modern medicines that animal laboratory-houses were first established – the earliest ones were in universities that taught medicine or veterinary science. These animals were largely used in anatomical investigations. The publication of The Origin of Species by Charles Darwin in 1858 drew the scientific communities’ attention to the fact that animals could make good models for understanding human biology. Only during the 20th century was the possibility of using drugs to cure many diseases fully realised. To do their medical research, to develop new drugs, scientists needed animals – often lots of them. The use of animals in experimentation had grown to such an extent by the 1920s that it was heavily criticised by Albert Schweitzer (1875–1965 – see below). In 1947, the Universities Federation for Animal Welfare published the first book on the management and care of laboratory animals. Today millions of animals are used each year for research in laboratory animal-houses, between three and four million in the UK alone. Laboratory animal-houses have improved greatly since the growing public awareness of animal welfare in the 1960s. However, the rate of improvement is not uniform across the globe as it tends to be society driven in those countries whose people express the most concern about animal welfare, e.g. western Europe. In the UK, the level of action against animal laboratories by animal-rights groups has forced most laboratories to be designed like fortresses, thereby denying animals the best housing conditions. For example, laboratory primates in the USA are regularly housed with extensive outdoor enclosures (Eichberg et al., 1991; Kessel & Brent, 2001). This is something that cannot be done in the UK because of animal-rights activists whose actions have included taking animals from laboratories, and even releasing mink (highly destructive predators) from farms into the British countryside.
The welfare problems of captive animals are often thought to be the product of modern systems of animal housing. We never imagine that beneath the Coliseum in Rome lions paced up and down in their tiny cells, or that sheep housed in a rock-walled pen chewed each others wool, or even that the Chinese emperor’s pet pekinese howled when left alone. However, animal welfare scientists know that if we recreated historical housing conditions for farm, zoo or pet animals, these animals would suffer welfare problems. Unfortunately, we have no direct evidence of the level of animal-welfare experienced by animals more than a few hundred years ago. The best indirect evidence we have are teeth wear patterns from the skulls of several-thousand-year old horses – these wear patterns are identical to those produced by modern horses when crib-biting. However, it is difficult to prove categorically that these patterns were produced by crib-biting.

1.3 Two Approaches to Environmental Enrichment

The study and implementation of environmental enrich...

Table of contents

  1. Cover
  2. Contents
  3. The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare
  4. Title Page
  5. Copyright
  6. Preface
  7. Acknowledgements
  8. 1 Environmental Enrichment: an Historical Perspective
  9. 2 Why Bother with Environmental Enrichment?
  10. 3 Does Environmental Enrichment Work?
  11. 4 Proactive v. Reactive use of Environmental Enrichment
  12. 5 Designing an Enrichment Device
  13. 6 The Enrichment Programme
  14. 7 Enrichment for Different Categories of Animals
  15. 8 Food and Foraging Enrichment
  16. 9 Social Environmental Enrichment
  17. 10 Housing
  18. 11 Furniture, Toys and other Objects
  19. 12 Designing and Analysing Enrichment Studies
  20. 13 Information Sources about Environmental Enrichment
  21. References
  22. Glossary
  23. Index