Teaching Early Reading and Phonics
eBook - ePub

Teaching Early Reading and Phonics

Creative Approaches to Early Literacy

  1. 152 pages
  2. English
  3. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  4. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Teaching Early Reading and Phonics

Creative Approaches to Early Literacy

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

Learning to read is an exciting and vital part of every child's development.

The new edition of this book continues to provide trainees and teachers with a broad understanding of teaching reading and phonics, and equip them with the skills necessary to face the reality of the early years classroom in order to meet the needs of individual children.

With vital information on constructing relationships with young readers, and how to plan phonics within a rich, interactive and playful literacy pedagogy, the second edition now includes:

  • A brand new chapter on babies and early reading
  • More information on language acquisition and how children learn
  • A discussion of children with SEN
  • An appreciation for the rise of digital technologies in relation to reading

Whether you?re training to become a teacher, or already working in the classroom this book is ideal for those whowish to embed the teaching of phonics into carefully selected high quality materials - particularly in children?s literature.

Frequently asked questions

Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Yes, you can access Teaching Early Reading and Phonics by Kathy Goouch,Andrew Lambirth in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Education & Teaching Methods for Reading. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Year
2016
ISBN
9781473998438

1 Introduction Sensible approaches to teaching and reading

In this chapter, we aim to:
  • provide the context for the development of teaching early reading and phonics in the UK
  • draw together research and practice in the teaching of reading
  • introduce reading as a complex, social and cultural activity
  • claim that a broad and diverse range of children’s early experiences must be acknowledged, celebrated and used as a foundation for learning and reading
In today’s debates about teaching children to read can ‘wisdom prevail over nonsense, truth over falsehood’? (Goodman, 2014: 35)
In England, and for at least half a century, politics has wielded an inordinately heavy influence on the details of the policy and practice of the teaching of English in all its forms (Hall, 2004). More, politicians have consistently colonised the teaching of reading, infusing the discourse with reference to ‘common sense’ (Chew, 2006: 119), succumbing to powerful lobby groups (Wray, 2006; Barrs and Meek Spencer, 2007; Clark, 2014b) and excluding reference to rigorous research evidence (Wyse and Styles, 2007; Ellis and Moss, 2014). This has resulted in a very detailed statutory requirement that children’s attention should be focused on phonics instruction in the early years, and that fidelity to a systematic, synthetics phonics approach, ‘first, fast and only’, should persist (Rose, 2006; Ofsted, 2010, 2015). To determine that practice does not sway from this single focus, synthetic phonics is now embedded in statutory documentation (DfE, 2014) and is a major focus of the regulatory body, the Office for Standards in Education (Ofsted), in inspections. Additionally, a new phonics screening check, based on this approach and including ‘non-words’, was introduced in 2012 for all children at the end of Year 1, their first year in the primary sector. In 2015, Nick Gibb, the Minister for Schools, made a substantial claim in relation to the impact of this phonics screening check:
In 2012, 58% of pupils taking the check met the national standard. In 2013, it was 69% and by 2014, the proportion of pupils meeting the standard had risen to 74%: equivalent to 102,000 more 6-year-old children on track to read more effectively.
One hundred and twenty thousand more children are now on track to become excellent readers as a result of the government’s focus on phonics, vindicating reforms to transform the way young people learn to read.
For too long, thousands of young people have been allowed to fall behind in reading. This government’s drive to eliminate illiteracy is putting a stop to that. (Gibb, 2015)
Various critical responses have been made to the very many claims of this kind from the DfE, including, for example, that: the early test results were characterised by children attempting to make words out of ‘non-words’ and therefore failing; teachers are now able to train children not to attempt to make sense of individual words; it is possible (for parents and teachers) to train children to pass a test requiring the recitation of a list of words; reciting words is not reading; there is no evidence of children sustaining this and translating it into reading connected text as claimed (see, for example: NCB, 2011; Ellis and Moss, 2014; Goodman, 2014).
In public statements, media outputs and conference presentations, government ministers’ statements are prone either to conflate reading with decoding using synthetic phonics or to assume, without citing reliable evidence, that if children are trained to become proficient decoders of print, then other skills involved in learning to read will automatically follow, claiming for example that: ‘with success in the basics of decoding words, pupils will be able to move on to reading with increased fluency and speed, which will enable them to develop a love of reading for pleasure and the habit of reading for pleasure’ (Gibb, 2015).
In order to further ensure that the government’s phonics agenda is put into practice, no-notice Ofsted inspections for Faculties of Education in Universities have also been introduced, with inspectors required to evaluate the extent to which teacher trainees are able to: ‘teach early reading and demonstrate a clear understanding of systematic synthetic phonics, communication and language development’ (Ofsted 2014:32).
Failure to meet these requirements can have draconian consequences and, as Ellis and Moss have discovered, ‘may ultimately result in course funding being withdrawn’ (2014: 242). They claim that all universities now have to provide: ‘a minimum of 90 hours teaching the government-mandated approach to phonics and faculties who introduce student teachers to other approaches have received letters from the Department of Education reminding them of government policy’ (ibid.). It seems that, in England, Initial Teacher Training (ITT) in university faculties, local authorities, schools and individual teachers are all being assessed and held accountable for the ability of children who are six years old to decode a particular set of words and non-words when tested. The single-minded commitment to this approach has been further evidenced by the spending incurred by the government in support of the implementation of synthetic phonics programmes in schools, with apparently over £22 million spent by the government on materials and £1.3 million on training – funding which was supplied to schools as match funding and so doubling the amount spent specifically on establishing this model of phonics – between 2011 and 2013 (Clark, 2014b).
This privileging of the development of phonic skills, and specifically training in synthetic phonics, above other decoding and reading skills, is troubling in view of the range of research which challenges this approach (see, for example: Goswami and Bryant 1990; Hall, 2003, 2006; Ziegler and Goswami, 2006; Ellis, 2007; Shannon, 2007; Strauss and Altwerger, 2007; Wyse and Styles, 2007; Dombey, 2010; Goodman, 2014; Teale et al., 2014). Also troubling is the amount of money and resources that recent governments seem to have been prepared to invest, while consulting only with an elite, domain-specific group of academics (Hall et al, 2014) and without the support of rigorous and trustworthy research. Perhaps most worrying of all is that discussion, debate and dissent appears to be forbidden in this new politics of early education, within universities and schools, as successive governments in England, and elsewhere, are putting ‘all our literacy eggs into the phonics basket’ (Rosen, 2010: 2).

A sensible review of how children become literate

It is, of course, teachers in their everyday practice who are confronted with the real dilemmas created by political statements and Ofsted inspections and reports. One classic example of this is the following statement in the DfE’s argument for systematic, synthetic phonics as they make their additional supporting case for the use of simple decodable texts and resources:
When children are at the initial stage of mastering decoding, it is vital that they practise their decoding through the use of reading books which are consistent with their phonic knowledge. A child who has yet to study the digraph ‘ph’ should not be asked to read a book about an ‘elephant’ – they are likely to find this confusing and frustrating. (DfE, 2015: 19)
In the above example, there is an authoritative statement that mastery of decoding is an essential prerequisite for a child to recognise and make meaning from a complex word such as ‘elephant’, a confident assertion frequently made and so quickly assumed into policy, and public, discourse. At first glance, this seems to be incontestable – common sense. However, the term ‘decoding’ is often mistakenly used synonymously with phonemic deconstruction of text, where individual words are broken down into single phonemic units of sound. But ‘decoding’ may be associated with a range of interpretative skills and children frequently ‘decode’ a text through the use of many strategies – including semantic and syntactic cues, as well as by using other bibliographic prompts. Such strategies, though, are ‘forbidden’ in Ofsted criteria as ‘children should not be expected to use strategies such as whole-word recognition and/or cues from context, grammar or pictures’ (Ofsted, 2010: 43), and so the use of the term ‘decoding’ is somewhat misleading. It is commonly accepted that the English language is complex and irregular (for clear and rigorous explanations, see the detailed work of: Goswami 2002, 2005, 2007; and Dombey, 2011) and over-dependence on phonemic recognition alone will confuse children who are used to learning in an eclectic way, using all of their senses and the resources available to them. Of course, anecdotally, we could all cite examples of very young children who not only can recognise the print word ‘elephant’ in a recognisable context but can also distinguish and articulate the complex and phonically highly irregular names of dinosaurs – because they are interesting.
However, in order to further question this claim by Ofsted that children should not be given a book about elephants before mastering the digraph ‘ph’, this introduction and book moves away from the well-trodden path of critiquing the Clackmannanshire study, which the government has wholeheartedly endorsed, frequently quoted and used without question to support their agenda, and which has been rigorously analysed and robustly challenged by many (see, for example: Goswami, 2007; Wyse and Goswami, 2008; Ellis and Moss, 2014). Instead, the importance of children’s early literacy induction, pre-school learning and early literacy learning needs to be robustly acknowledged and offered in support of how children can best be taught in their early years at school and how they can be motivated to become volitional readers, rather than functional decoders of print. Specifically, significant focus needs to be on the importance of naturally occurring, affective, sometimes informal conversational opportunities in order to develop language, increase vocabulary and support meaning-making; the importance of developing relationships with significant ‘others’ as they develop semiotic knowledge and symbolic understanding; and the part that environment and resources, including books, plays in early reading development. In this book, we stress the importance of teachers’ knowledge about children, research and literacy development; the importance of talk and conversation; and the importance of places, environments and resources.
In the first two years of language acquisition, children progress rapidly from learning about one new word every week, to one a day and then to one word every one to two waking hours (Tomasello, 2003: 50). Tomasello also points out that there are frequently accelerations of word acquisition and these may occur when children are involved in a kind of oral close procedure – as they become familiar with more vocabulary, they are able to identify more new words in the flow of speech that they hear, which combines with their developing knowledge of the functions of language in communicative events. Thus, a social induction to language occurs, in the company of familiar contexts, resources and, importantly, significant adults. However, over-simplistic political reference to the number of words known by children as they begin school denies the potential richness of vocabulary and experience that children may have experienced and on which effective school literacy practices may effectively be constructed.

And elephants?

What can be learned from these early literacy learning experiences that will support and motivate children to learn to read and to read for pleasure? Who are the wise ‘experts’ in relation to how children learn to read? What counts as knowledge and whose knowledge counts?
Early learning (from at least birth) really does matter and it seems more than ever important to acknowledge the huge amount of learning already undertaken by children in the first 48 months of their life, much of which is relevant to beginning reading. This includes: meaning construction; hearing, identifying, remembering and repeating patterned language; participating in the semiotic world; knowledge of symbolic referencing and an interest and enjoyment in books and stories, images and print text; and developing knowledge of the alphabetic symbols and some associated sounds. However, in order to give significance to this early learning, teachers, managers, policy-makers and politicians need to understand the complexity that is reading and the early development, learning and behaviours that contribute to children becoming readers – as:
an excessive zealotry for one version of phonics ignores a fundamental truth about reading: that it is essentially to do with the construction of meaning in the reader’s mind, on the basis of the evidence provided by marks on a page or a screen. (Richmond et al., 2015:7)
All of us who are involved directly or indirectly in children’s experiences of learning to become readers need to acknowledge ‘the hypothesis-forming, rule-testing, rule-adapting, memory-employing, meaning-making complex activity which is reading’ (ibid.) and these skills are developing in children, and are learned in homes and families, from birth.
The importance of affect in relation to all learning and reading needs to be loudly acknowledged. This includes developing relationships with other readers (adults and peers); relationships with texts that matter; and the importance of positive interactions between companionable ‘others’, texts, the lived world and young developing readers. There is a mistaken belief that reading is an activity that must be undertaken individually, and tested individually. However, activities that involve ‘communities of readers’ (see, for example, Cremin et al., 2014) are engaging, affecting and frequently joyful, rather than silent and solitary, and for young developing readers this kind of ethos will be rewarding and will perpetuate reading as an enjoyable event, rather than as a school task to be overcome. Enjoyment of books, stories, texts of all kinds, needs to be a central part of the reading day for children from the beginning of their school lives, not a reward for overcoming phonic challenges in the early years.
Young children are already reading the word and the world before starting school. Young developing readers are encoding and decoding words and experiencing their effect (see for example, Grainger and Goouch, 1999; Price, 2000) as they hear how words sound and how they feel on their lips as they first learn to speak and then to read print texts. Acknowledgment that in the first four years of life babies and young children are absorbing and actively employing a number of complex messages about the world and about words – including how they are spoken and interpreted – seems essential in order to build on early literacy learning. Children are also learning about their place in the world through each event, either as active and responsive meaning-makers or as passive recipients of fragmentary learning opportunities. Supporting very young children in drawing together words and worlds is embedded in the role of parents, caring adults and then their teachers, who need to know the children in their care. Without doubt, ‘children are more likely to want to read material which connects to their personal interests, and may as a result discover what reading can offer them as individuals’ (Cremin et al., 2014). They might be interested in elephants.
The importance of quality texts simply cannot be overestimated. The word ‘elephant’ may matter to a developing reader more than more functional and easy decodable printed words and may have a meaning beyond the image of the word in a book. We know that ‘[children] learn when it’s inconvenient not to’ (‘Janette’ in Lambirth, 2007a: 80). Although the government has been informed through its commissioned review that the ‘language of written texts is accessed via the eyes rather than the ears’ (Rose, 2006: appendix, n62), the work of neuroscience offers different explanations and offers clear information that is relevant to reading experiences, at all ages. For example, ‘it is now well established that visual signals are not just relayed passively into the deep recesses of the brain and up into the cortex’ (Greenfield, 2000:65). This seems to challenge any idea that there is a simple route from the printed word to a memory bank of words. It seems that our brains...

Table of contents

  1. Cover
  2. Half Title
  3. Publisher Note
  4. Title Page
  5. Copyright Page
  6. Acknowledgements
  7. Contents
  8. About the Authors
  9. 1 Introduction Sensible approaches to teaching and reading
  10. 2 Beginning with Babies
  11. 3 Critical Contexts
  12. 4 The Role of the Teacher
  13. 5 Knowledge for Reading
  14. 6 Environments for Reading
  15. 7 Resources for Reading
  16. 8 Reading Routines
  17. 9 Talk, Reading and Writing
  18. 10 Assessment of Reading
  19. 11 Conclusions Principles and Practice
  20. References
  21. Index