Evidence-based Investigative Interviewing
eBook - ePub

Evidence-based Investigative Interviewing

Applying Cognitive Principles

Jason J. Dickinson, Nadja Schreiber Compo, Rolando Carol, Bennett L. Schwartz, Michelle McCauley, Jason J. Dickinson, Nadja Schreiber Compo, Rolando Carol, Bennett L. Schwartz, Michelle McCauley

  1. 262 pages
  2. English
  3. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  4. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Evidence-based Investigative Interviewing

Applying Cognitive Principles

Jason J. Dickinson, Nadja Schreiber Compo, Rolando Carol, Bennett L. Schwartz, Michelle McCauley, Jason J. Dickinson, Nadja Schreiber Compo, Rolando Carol, Bennett L. Schwartz, Michelle McCauley

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

For as long as we have been researching human memory, psychologists have been investigating how people remember and forget. This research is regularly drawn upon in our legal systems. Historically, we have relied upon eyewitness memory to help judge responsibility and adjudicate truth, but memory is malleable, prone to error, and susceptible to bias. Even confident eyewitnesses make mistakes, and even accurate witnesses sometimes find their testimony subjected to harsh scrutiny.

Emerging from this environment, the Cognitive Interview (CI) became a means of assisting cooperative witnesses with recalling more information without sacrificing accuracy. First used by police interviewing adult witnesses, it is now used with many populations in many contexts, including public health, accident reconstruction, and the interrogation of terror suspects. Evidence-Based Investigative Interviewing reviews the application of cognitive research to investigative interviewing, revealing how principles of cognition, memory, and social dynamics may increase the accuracy of eyewitness testimony. It provides evidence-based applications for investigators beyond the forensic domain in areas such as eyewitness identification, detecting deception, and interviewing children.

Drawing together the work of thirty-three authors across both the academic and practice communities, this comprehensive collection is essential reading for researchers in psychology, forensics, and disciplines such as epidemiology and gerontology.

Frequently asked questions

Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Yes, you can access Evidence-based Investigative Interviewing by Jason J. Dickinson, Nadja Schreiber Compo, Rolando Carol, Bennett L. Schwartz, Michelle McCauley, Jason J. Dickinson, Nadja Schreiber Compo, Rolando Carol, Bennett L. Schwartz, Michelle McCauley in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Derecho & Teoría y práctica del derecho. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Publisher
Routledge
Year
2019
ISBN
9781351662000
1
Expanding the Cognitive Interview to Non-Criminal Investigations
Ronald P. Fisher and R. Edward Geiselman
Introduction
Information is the lifeblood of many investigations, whether about crime, accidents, public health, national security, or a host of others. Some of that information may result from analysis of physical records (e.g., instruments, a patient’s medical chart, skid marks, explosive devices, the plane’s black box), and some will come from interviewing people who have first-hand knowledge of the event (e.g., pilots, soldiers, physicians, victims, bystander witnesses, informants). The present chapter focuses on eliciting information from human sources.
On the surface, it might seem that if interviewers are knowledgeable about the event to be investigated, i.e., they know what kind of information to elicit (e.g., the robber’s appearance, the train’s speed, which terrorists attended a meeting) it should be relatively easy to extract that information – assuming, of course, that the interviewee is motivated to provide the requested information (see Brandon & Wells, this volume, Leins & Zimmerman, this volume for interview techniques with uncooperative respondents). Thus, for instance, if a police investigator needs to find out the criminal’s ethnicity, the police officer/interviewer can simply ask: Was he White, Black, Hispanic, or Asian? And, in fact, that is how many investigative interviewers proceed. That is, investigators think in terms of content (the information to be gathered) and then ask specific questions targeting each to-be-gathered fact, e.g., how fast was the car going? (Fisher, Geiselman, Raymond, Jurkevich, & Warhaftig, 1987). Although this method of interviewing (asking specific questions) is common among police – and others – it is not an effective method to gather information from human sources. Geiselman and Fisher therefore set out to devise an alternative method of interviewing, one that revolved around witnesses’ mental processes (rather than around the facts to be gathered) to elicit more information from human sources. The resulting procedure, called the Cognitive Interview (CI), was born out of this need, to devise an interview process based on the witness’s and the interviewer’s psychological processes.
The original version of the CI was developed specifically for criminal investigation. That the CI took this direction was mainly happenstance. It emanated from a brief conversation we had in Ed Geiselman’s office about what profession could best profit from applying psychological researchers’ knowledge about memory-enhancing techniques. Ed’s immediate thought was that police could benefit most from enhancing witness memory, because solving crimes depends mainly on witness evidence – despite the more exotic, but uncommon, solutions often found on television shows. And so, we devised the CI to be used by police to enhance witness recall of crime-related events.
When we first examined the problem, of witnesses failing to remember key elements of a crime, we thought about it mainly as a witness memory problem. As such, the original version of the CI (Geiselman et al., 1984) was composed exclusively of techniques that we believed would enhance witnesses retrieving information about past experiences (see Table 1.1: Active respondent participation; Report everything; Varied Retrieval). We later realized that witnesses’ under-reporting criminal events was not only a reflection of their using inefficient memory-retrieval mechanisms, but also that the interviewers were not processing information efficiently (e.g., listening to and notating the witness’s statements, keeping track of questions to be asked, developing a hypothesis of how the crime occurred, etc.). Moreover, other non-memory factors also contributed to witnesses’ under-reporting events, including the social dynamics between the interviewer and the witness (e.g., inadequate rapport), and the difficulty of communicating some ideas (e.g., describing the odor of a fire). Thus, whereas the initial version of the CI was a simple collection of memory-enhancing mnemonics, the CI evolved into a more wide-ranging approach (the so-called “Enhanced CI”) that addresses (a) the social dynamics between the interviewer and the witness, (b) the witness’s memory, and other cognitive processes, and also the interviewer’s cognitive processes, and (c) the witness and the interviewer communicating effectively with one another. The core elements of the CI and the psychological processes we intended to enhance are presented in Table 1.1. (For a complete description of the CI, see Fisher & Geiselman, 1992.)
Table 1.1 Elements of the Cognitive Interview
CI Element Description Psychological processes enhanced1
Rapport Develop rapport between respondent and interviewer Social Dynamics
Active respondent participation Respondent actively to generate information (not merely to answer) Interviewer’s questions) Social Dynamics
Report everything Include all recollections in response; do not edit out unimportant details2 Memory & Communication
Reinstate context Reinstate the context of the original experience Memory
Describe in detail Instruct respondents to provide a detailed account3 Communication
Close eyes Instruct respondents to close their eyes4 Cognition
No interruptions Do not interrupt the respondent’s narration5 Social Dynamics & Cognition
Don’t guess Instruct respondents not to guess (OK to say “I don’t know”) Cognition
Open-ended questions Ask primarily open-ended questions (closed questions as follow-up) Social Dynamics & Cognition
Multiple retrieval Encourage respondents to search through memory more than once Memory
Varied retrieval Encourage respondents to search through memory in different ways Memory
Respondent- compatible questions Ask questions that are compatible with respondent’s currently accessible information Memory
Avoid suggestive questions Avoid asking questions that suggest a specific answer Memory
Code-compatible output Allow respondents to output their knowledge in the same form as it is stored (often non-verbal) Communication
1These are the psychological processes we intended to enhance. We do not know which psychological processes were actually enhanced. Some of the CI elements were intended to influence more than one process.
2Sometimes this is misinterpreted to mean that respondents are encouraged to guess. Respondents are not encouraged to guess (see “Don’t guess” instruction). This instruction encourages respondents to report all facts, whether the respondents consider the facts important or not.
3This instruction can be embellished by the interviewer providing an example of a model (detailed) statement (see Leal, Vrij, Warmelink, Vernham, & Fisher, 2015).
4Asking respondents to close their eyes should be implemented only after rapport has been developed.
5This suggestion is relaxed if the respondent’s narrative goes far afield of the critical event.
As is documented in other chapters in this book and also in published reviews and meta-analyses (e.g., Fisher & Geiselman, 2018; Köhnken, Milne, Memon, & Bull, 1999; Memon, Meissner, & Fraser, 2010), the CI has been extremely successful in eliciting more information from witnesses to crime. The present chapter explores how the CI can be adapted to other, non-criminal investigations. Because the CI is a purely process-oriented approach, we expected – in truth, we hoped – that its effectiveness would extend to other kinds of investigation that depend on the same underlying psychological processes that the CI was intended to bolster, namely, (a) the social dynamics between the interviewer and the respondent, (b) the memory and cognitive processes of the respondent and the interviewer, and (c) communication between the interviewer and the respondent. Clearly, there are many kinds of investigative interviews that depend on these three underlying psychological processes, and which may benefit from the CI, including, debriefing military combatants after a mission, gathering human intelligence information from informants, interviewing drivers and passengers about vehicular accidents or interviewing workers about industrial accidents, interviewing patients about their medical histories, etc.
In exploring these non-criminal applications of the CI, we report both our own research efforts and also those of our colleagues, several of whom are contributors to this book. Most of these research studies, naturally, were conducted in controlled, laboratory environments with college students as the respondents in a simulated situation. Some research studies, however, were also conducted outside the laboratory, in the field, where the experimental participants were “real people” engaging in their normal, real-world (non-laboratory) lives. Finally, we describe a few instances in which high-ranking professional investigators incorporated elements of the CI into their investigations of real-world critical events.
Survey of Empirical Research
We take as our jump-off point into reporting the research on non-criminal events the most basic kind of experience: A single, external event that occurs in a specific episodic context (time and place) and the respondent is a cooperative, untrained observer. Within this basic experience, we examine both low-arousal events (e.g., memory for conversations) and high-arousal events (e.g., traffic accidents). From there, we explore events that vary systematically from the basic experience, either because they are collections of many similar events or because the respondent is a trained observer (e.g., police officer). We then examine a more common experience, in which a person’s goal is to extract meaning (e.g., listening to a story). Finally, we explore internal, mental events (e.g., decisions, plans, and emotions), and we speculate on the CI’s use in a therapeutic context.
Before presenting the experimental findings, let us describe the typical research paradigm, including the different variations. Generally, experimental participants experience an event, and then are interviewed about their memory for the event. The researcher typically measures how much the interviewee recalls and the accuracy of his/her recollection. Within this standard procedure there is considerable variation: (a) The experimental participants are usually college students, but in some studies they are young children, or older or non-student adults. (b) The experienced event is usually shown on videotape but sometimes it is experienced live; generally the participants observe the critical event passively, but sometimes they actively participate in the event; the event is usually fashioned by the experimenter, but in some studies, it is a naturally-occurring event from the participant’s life. (c) The interviews are usually conducted immediately or within minutes of the critical event, but in some studies, several days, months, or years intervene. (d) The interview is conducted either as a CI or a “control” interview, which can either be a “Structured Interview” (a technique that incorporates generally accepted principles of interviewing, but does not include the memory-enhancing techniques unique to the CI) or a procedure that simulates a typical police interview. Many of the studies that claimed to conduct the CI used only some, but not all, of the CI elements (see Table 1.1), and so it is important when reading the individual studies to note exactly which CI elements were used. We address this issue later in the chapter (Retrospective Comments on the CI Research).
Single, external events
Low-arousal events: Laboratory studies using the CI have examined, among others, memory for innocuous events like blood donations and health-related events.
Blood donations: In some of the earliest studies on the CI, Köhnken and colleagues assessed the original version of the CI in a blood donation event. Experimental participants were shown a videotape of a typical blood donation event and then were interviewed with either a CI or a Structured Interview. In both Köhnken, Thürer, and Zoberbier (1994) and Mantwill, Köhnken, and Aschermann (1995), interviewees remembered more blood donation details when interviewed with the CI than with the Structured Interview, and at comparable levels of accuracy. This occurred both for participants who had donated blood in the past and for those who had not donated in the past (Mantwill et al., 1995). In Köhnken et al. (1994), not only were the interviewees tested for recall of the blood donation event, but also the interviewers were tested on their recollections of what the interviewees reported during the interview. Interviewers who had conducted a CI recalled more (of what the interviewees had reported) than interviewers who conducted a Structured Interview. Thus the CI not only facilitated observers’ recollection of an external event, but also the interviewers’ recollections of what transpired during the interview.
Accidents: A more common experience that should lend itself to the CI is one in which the witness is a passive bystander observer of either a vehicular or industrial accident. The CI was examined in at least four laboratory studies in which experimental participants watched a video of a car accident (Brock, Fisher, & Cutler, 1999; Chapman & Perry, 1995; Ginet & Verkampt, 2007; Milne, Clare, & Bull, 1999) and one in which the witnesses viewed a video of an industrial accident (MacLean, Stinson, Kelloway, & Fisher, 2011). In all four car accident studies, the CI elicited considerably more information than a Structured Interview, and at comparable accuracy rates. In the one study of memory for an industrial accident, the results were mixed, with the CI eliciting more information than the control interview, but at a lower accuracy rate. Given that the information-processing requirements for watching a video of an industrial accident should be similar to those of watching a car accident, what accounts for the different findings? We suspect that the more encouraging results of the car accident studies reflected that the CI and control interviews were conducted as interactive, face-to-face interviews, whereas the mixed results of the industrial accident reflected that the “interview” was a pre-printed set of instructions to which the witnesses wrote a written response. Moreover, in this study, several of the CI prompts were closed multiple-choice questions, which are prone to error and therefore generally discouraged when conducting a CI. Finally, a critical element of the CI, instructing witnesses not to guess if unsure, was missing from the written CI instructions. We will describe shortly, when discussing high-arousal events, a more compelling examination of the CI in which the witnesses were victims of real car accidents (Ginet, Teissedre, Verkampt, & Fisher, 2016).
Conversations: Relatively little research has been conducted on recalling conversations. It is unfortunate that memory for conversations has been overlooked, given its applied value in several domains: For national security, interviewers may want to learn what transpired during meetings of terrorist groups; business leaders or attorneys may want to know who said what during high-level business discussions; in current vogue in Washington politics, investigators want to know about conversations between American government officials and representatives of foreign governments. At least four studies have been conducted to examine the CI’s effect on participants’ recollections of earlier conversations. In Campos and Alonso-Quecuty (2008), Spanish college-aged students watched a video of two criminals discussing in detail their plans to commit a crime. The participants attempted to remember the conversation 15 minutes later, when they participated in either a CI or an interview modeled after a typical Spanish police interview. The CI-intervi...

Table of contents

  1. Cover
  2. Half Title
  3. Title Page
  4. Copyright Page
  5. Contents
  6. List of Illustrations
  7. Notes on Contributors
  8. Foreword
  9. 1 Expanding the Cognitive Interview to Non-Criminal Investigations
  10. 2 The Impact of the Cognitive Interview in the UK and Recent Research in Portugal
  11. 3 Expanding the Legacy of the Cognitive Interview: Developments and Innovations in Evidence-Based Investigative Interviewing
  12. 4 The Cognitive Interview: A Tiered Approach in the Real World
  13. 5 State of Intoxication: A Review of the Effects of Alcohol on Witnesses’ Memory
  14. 6 From the Police Station to the Hospital Bed: Using the Cognitive Interview to Enhance Epidemiologic Interviewing
  15. 7 The Verifiability Approach
  16. 8 Commonalities and Complementarities Among Science-Based Interview Methods: Towards a Theory of Interrogation
  17. 9 Navigating the Interview: Judgment and Decision Making in Investigative Interviewing
  18. 10 Investigative Interviews with Adult Sexual Assault Complainants: Challenges and Future Directions
  19. 11 Techniques for Interviewing Reluctant Child Witnesses
  20. 12 Investigative Interviewing About Repeated Experiences
  21. 13 Viewing the Cognitive Interview Through the Lens of Self-Determination Theory
  22. Index