Exceptionality in East Asia
eBook - ePub

Exceptionality in East Asia

Explorations in the Actiotope Model of Giftedness

  1. 304 pages
  2. English
  3. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  4. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Exceptionality in East Asia

Explorations in the Actiotope Model of Giftedness

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

The continual successes of students from East-Asia are confirmed in a variety of international tests of academic achievement and yet, despite this attainment, many scholars have realised that a substantial proportion of these students are also underachieving.

Using the actiotope model of giftedness to integrate a broad range of research, this innovative book features a number of chapters written by internationally recognised scholars in a frank and lively discussion about the origins of exceptionality in students from East Asia. With the actiotope model as the theoretical framework, the book distinguishes between trait models of giftedness and systems approaches to exceptionality. Breaking new ground in understanding the complex interactions between a learner's environment, goals, intelligence and motivations in the development of their ever-expanding knowledge and skill set, this book will:



  • describe, with examples, a systems approach to the development of exceptionality, allowing educators and researchers the ability to track students with greater precision;


  • influence the means by which educators identify and support students with the potential for exceptional performance;


  • suggest possible reasons for the variability in the achievement of potentially gifted students;


  • provide strategies to support these students;


  • have a profound effect on the way that exceptionality and giftedness are defined and understood, not only in East Asia but also in the West.

Covering issues that have firm theoretical foundations and which are based on cutting edge ideas, Exceptionality in East Asia has significant implications for gifted education and is essential reading for scholars, undergraduate and postgraduate students interested in the psychological and social basis of exceptionality.

Frequently asked questions

Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Yes, you can access Exceptionality in East Asia by Shane N. Phillipson,Heidrun Stoeger,Albert Ziegler in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Education & Education General. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Publisher
Routledge
Year
2013
ISBN
9781136455711
Edition
1
Chapter 1
The Actiotope Model of Giftedness
An Introduction to some Central Theoretical Assumptions
Albert Ziegler, Wilma Vialle and
Bastian Wimmer
Scenario 1: Favela Rocinha in the south of Rio de Janeiro. Little Carlos is sitting on three piled-up tyres. The four chairs around the only table in the wooden hut are occupied by his oldest brother and his friend playing cards together.
Scenario 2: 155th street, Holocombe Rucker Playground, in the middle of a neighborhood in the poorest part of Harlem. Mike, aged eight, is dreaming of doing one ‘slam dunk’ after another some day during the ‘Rucker’, the world’s most famous street basketball tournament.
Scenario 3: The room of Lian, a third-grade pupil. She’s going to do a mathematics test in two weeks, but cannot decide if she should start studying or watch a TV show which is very popular among her classmates.
It may seem unlikely to us that Carlos is going to be a professional card player, that Mike is going to be a professional basketball player or that Lian is going to be a great mathematician. But how could this scenario change?
Effective Action Repertoires
These are three scenarios, representing three totally different worlds of actions and opportunities for personal development. Conventional models of talent propose that the key to answering this question lies in the special personality traits of the three children. These models label them with talents, gifts, abilities, and so on (Shavinina, 2009; Sternberg and Davidson, 2005). By contrast, the actiotope model of giftedness emphasizes the dynamic interaction of individuals with the environment. The focus of interest under the actiotope model, then, is action not traits. All humans have a different action repertoire – that is, the possibilities for acting – which they could realize in principle. Carlos, for example, can play cards in a very sophisticated way for a boy of his age. Mike scores the most points with a basketball compared to his friends and Lian masters arithmetical operations that would normally be expected of children two years older than her. Although these three youngsters show remarkable performance in special fields for their age, their action repertoire is not comparable to that of an expert in his or her special field. Experts have a far more effective action repertoire, which differs on at least seven characteristics from the repertoires of actions of Carlos, Mike or Lian (Ericsson, 1998; Ericsson, Charness, Feltovich and Hoffman, 2006). These seven characteristics comprise the following:
  1. The action repertoire of experts in their specialty includes actions that are more successful. A professional musician who is asked to play a new track will immediately find a much better interpretation than a good amateur musician. Similarly, chess Grand Masters, who analyze chess patterns, find much stronger turns than a novice chess player. Mathematics professors can solve complex equation systems with ease; the average person finds them very dif-ficult to understand.
  2. The action repertoire of experts in their specialty is far more elaborated. Chess Grand Masters, for example, have as many useful chess patterns (chunks) saved in their mind as there are words in their native language. They record more items of information, recognize the diverse relationships among them, and save those items in a more structured manner.
  3. Access to effective actions. Experts have sophisticated strategies, enabling them to retrieve successful actions and solutions to problems more quickly and in a more targeted manner. By contrast, the novice has access to poor choices along with possibilities for success. If you have managed to ride a bike without falling off, for example, there is no guarantee that the next time you ride a bike you will also be free of accidents.
  4. Analysis of problems. Before experts act, they analyze the problem extensively and create a more action–functional problem representation than does a novice.
  5. Physical adaptations. The bodies of experts are adjusted in many ways to the requirements of their domain. To illustrate, this applies not only to the different muscular systems of weightlifters, table tennis players and radiologists, but also to the specialized regions of their brains, which are enlarged in connection to their activities.
  6. Strategies. Experts use more suitable strategies to arrive at a solution. Expert physicians, for example, start with the given information and work their way through to the solution of the problem. Students of physics, however, typically reverse this process and try to work their way backwards from the unknown, to the given information.
  7. Cognitive effort. Experts have automated an enormous number of cognitive action steps. They do not have to be laboriously constructed to solve a problem, but can simply be retrieved. Consequently, cognitive resources are available for the analysis of aspects of problems, whose solutions are unknown at this point.
Taken together, these characteristics explain why experts, with their effective action repertoires, are superior to the average capable person in their special field. However, these findings do not explain why some people are able to develop from an ordinary action repertoire to an extraordinarily effective action repertoire. Do talents and gifts actually play the critical role that conventional giftedness researchers believe?
Intelligence Adaptations
If giftedness researchers want to assess whether Carlos, Mike or Lian could ever build up an exceptionally effective action repertoire, they usually want to examine whether these three children bring talents and gifts with them. In the actiotope model, this question is regarded as less scientific. The question that is asked is whether all three can learn in an effective way; which enables them to build up an excellent action repertoire in card playing, basketball or mathematics. The answer is given within the scope of the systemic paradigm. The fundamental theoretical unit on which all analyses are based is the actiotope.
What is an Actiotope?
The focus of the actiotope approach is action and the possibilities for acting possessed by individuals. These can only be understood if we recognize them as a result of three adaptations:
  • a biological adaptation that was mainly carried by the human species and is conceptually locatable in biotopes;
  • a social adaptation that is mainly carried by social associations, which we can conceptually locate in sociotopes; and
  • an individual adaptation that is carried by individuals, which we can conceptually locate in actiotopes.
Essentially all actions, which are of interest in research on giftedness, are a result of these three adaptations. If Carlos puts a card on the table, for example, he does it with his hand (not with a fin or a wing); this is a consequence of biological adaptation (in biotopes). Both the card game itself and its rules are the results of a social adaptation (in sociotopes). The choice of particular card moves is a result of Carlos’s individual adaptation (which happens in his actiotope). In a similar vein, all of Lian’s arithmetic skills are based upon enormous developments in the information processing of the vertebrates (in biotopes), the development of the mathematics discipline (in sociotopes) and the individual development of her arithmetic skills (in her actiotope).
In line with these introductory remarks, we can define an actiotope thus:
An actiotope includes an individual and the material, social and informational environment with which that individual actively interacts.
Three Perspectives on the Actiotope
Every actiotope is unique. In every actiotope, the individual can access a range of special possibilities for interacting with his or her environment. Every environment sets different conditions for success. When faced with a particular environment and the possibilities of learning which that environment affords, individuals construct their action repertoire.
Action repertoires always have functionality that is unique to the actiotope in which the individual is currently acting. Hence, actiotopes are conceptual analytical units, in which the individual’s acting and the possibilities of actions within the environment are integrated. Instead of single personal traits like talents or gifts, the actiotope approach examines individuals and their individual world of actions.1 System-theoretical considerations are significant from three perspectives:
  • The component perspective (What are the elements of an actiotope? How do they interact?).
  • The dynamic perspective (How do actiotopes change?).
  • The system perspective (How do actiotopes remain stable, especially as they develop into excellence?).
Component Perspective
All actions show four characteristics. The action in question:
  • is part of the action repertoire of the person;
  • pursues an aim that seems reachable because of this action;
  • is made possible because the situation was constituted in a way to allow this action; and,
  • is selected because the person decided that the action was the most expedient in this situation from the repertoire of possible actions.
Based on this analysis of actions, the actiotope approach distinguishes four components:
  1. The action repertoire is the total of individual possibilities of actions (e.g. first grade pupils typically can add and multiply in their heads, while fourth grade pupils can also calculate the same operations in a notational way. Hence, the latter’s mathematical action repertoire is more comprehensive.).
  2. Goals, which are targeted conditions by the individual through actions (e.g. learning targets, social aims, professional goals).
  3. Environment—the material, social and informational environment with which an individual actively interacts (i.e. within the actiotope) as well as the external environments of an actiotope.
  4. Subjective action space, which are the possibilities of actions considered by the individual (to reach the aims, the most promising actions in this situation are chosen from the personal action repertoire—e.g. in a basketball match Mike may dribble around his opponent using the right instead of the left side; Carlos may try a bluff in his first card game for money when he has a bad set of cards; and Lian may choose an indirect mathematical proof for a mathematical theorem).
As the bracketed examples suggest, the four components are not only involved in the accomplishment of excellent performance actions, but are also constituents of every action, especially for learning actions.
While points 1 (action repertoire, or what may be viewed as competencies) and 2 (goals) have been explored within the field of gifted education, the environment and the subjective space of actions have not received adequate theoretical examination. In this chapter, only the the role of environment is discussed further.
A Proposal to Analyze Environment: Sociotopes
To study the learning efficiency of environments, Ziegler (2008, 2009) suggested the construct of sociotopes. Sociotopes are relatively stable configurations of the environment, which exert stable influences on the actions of individuals. Spoken in system theoretical jargon, sociotopes are control variables (Thelen and Smith, 2006 ).
Some conceptual preliminary remarks are important. First, sociotopes are understood as a framework condition of tangible actions. Second, they are conceived in a specific way in view of learning possibilities (e.g. an environment may be a learning sociotope for playing football, but not for mathematics). Third, the relativity of sociotopes has to be considered (i.e. the same spatial environment can be a totally different sociotope for different individuals).
A sociotope concept, which fulfils these conditions, conceives environments not as a geographical area, but as a space of action for individuals. Thereby an objective action space and a normative action space are distinguished (Ziegler, 2011). Lian’s learning should serve as an example. This case is concerned with actions in mathematics, which literally means that both objective and normative actions are conceived in view of mathematical actions (e.g. learning maths, doing a calculation, watching a movie about the life of a mathematician, talking about maths, and so on).
Under objective action space, the total of possibilities of actions is understood as those that can be theoretically executed in a given situation. Classrooms, train compartments, slopes, swimming pools, and so on, can offer these totally different objective action spaces of actions.
Within the normative space of actions, every possible action from the point of view of the individual may either be desirable, undesirable or without any normative valence.2 That implies the classification illustrated in Table 1.1 (cf. Ziegler, 2008; 2009).
Table 1.1 Classification of sociotopes in the domain of mathematics
Learning Sociotope
Here, learning is possible and desired. Learning sociotopes can be a classroom during lessons, or completing homework at a desk at home. Additionally, they can include situations for learning outside the school context, such as extra-curricular music lessons, language courses, or sports training. These represent all the environments in which Lian’s mathematical learning is desired. There is a good case to believe that she is in more of these mathematical learning sociotopes than Carlos or Mike. Those individuals would be more frequently in the learning sociotopes in which they could improve in card playing or basketball respectively.
Infrastructural Sociotopes
These also allow performance gain and learning, but the accomplishment of learning actions is voluntary. Learning mathematics, for example, is something that Lian can also do on a park bench, during a bus or train ride, with an opened booklet at the breakfast table or by using mathematical knowledge offerings featured in the media. In Mike’s case every backyard with a basketball hoop can be an infrastructural sociotope. A group of card-playing friends, who are not playing to improve, can be an infrastructural sociotope for Carlos.
Avoidance Sociotype
In an avoidance sociotope, learning is possible, but not desired. It is possible that Lian wants to use a recess break, spare time at school, or a day off from school to learn mathematics, but she encounters criticism from her classmates and/or her parents. She is then placed in the awkward situation of having to justify her wish to learn. Equally, Mike and his friends could be told...

Table of contents

  1. Cover
  2. Half Title
  3. Title Page
  4. Copyright
  5. Dedication
  6. Contents
  7. List of contributors
  8. Foreword
  9. Preface
  10. 1. The actiotope model of giftedness: an introduction to some central theoretical assumptions
  11. 2. Talent development as adaptation: the role of educational and learning capital
  12. 3. Confucianism, learning self-concept and the development of exceptionality
  13. 4. Pathways to artistic giftedness: developmental theory as a complement to the actiotope model of giftedness
  14. 5. Chinese students and mathematical problem solving: an application of the actiotope model of giftedness
  15. 6. Intelligence and academic achievement – with a focus on the actiotope model of giftedness
  16. 7. Goal orientations and the development of subjective action space in Chinese students
  17. 8. Social-emotional development of Chinese gifted students: a review based within the actiotope model of giftedness
  18. 9. The “Tiger Mother” factor: curriculum, schooling and mentoring of Asian students in an Australian context
  19. 10. Parental involvement within the actiotope model of giftedness: what it means for East-Asian students
  20. 11. Support-oriented identification of gifted students in East Asia according to the actiotope model of giftedness
  21. 12. Twice-exceptional students with deafness or hard-of-hearing and giftedness
  22. 13. Gifted education policy and the development of exceptionality: a Hong Kong perspective
  23. 14. The gifted and talented and effective learning: a focus on the actiotope model of giftedness in the Asian context
  24. Index