Sensory Research
eBook - ePub

Sensory Research

Multimodal Perspectives

  1. 352 pages
  2. English
  3. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  4. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Sensory Research

Multimodal Perspectives

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

This volume is a record of the proceedings of a festspiel held to honor Jozef F. Zwislocki for his outstanding contributions to science and to Syracuse University. His contributions to the knowledge of the hydromechanical, neurophysiological, and perceptual mechanisms of the auditory system are truly monumental. In addition, his contributions to the comprehension of the mammalian auditory system include not only landmark ideas, but also many of the experimental findings in psychoacoustics and peripheral auditory physiology that constitute the database which has provided a springboard for research in laboratories throughout the world. His efforts to link physics, biology, and psychophysics to create a basis for our understanding of the nervous system have had an influence that extends far beyond the science of acoustics. Although the purpose of this conference was to recognize the many achievements of Professor Zwislocki, the spirit of the participants was to honor him in a manner that best characterized his lifetime dedication to research, that is, to report the results of their own work. Consequently, this volume is first and foremost a compilation of scientific papers in the area of sensory research. Some are reports of recent experiments and some present an overview of research efforts extending from the past up to ongoing work. His influence can be recognized in all of the contributions and some explicitly describe the ties between their own work and the germinal ideas planted by him. This volume, in reflecting the rapid progress being made in sensory science and written by those who are making it, is a fitting tribute to Zwislocki, who always stood at the forefront of his science.

Frequently asked questions

Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Yes, you can access Sensory Research by Ronald T. Verrillo in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Psicología & Historia y teoría en psicología. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Year
2014
ISBN
9781317781301

1 Can Magnitude Scaling Reveal the Growth of Loudness in Cochlear Impairment?

Rhona P. Hellman
Northeastern University and Veterans Administration,

INTRODUCTION

Zwislocki’s interest and contribution to our understanding of the loudness-intensity relation were sparked by his early studies with Lüscher (Lüscher & Zwislocki, 1948a, 1948b, 1951) and the development of his comprehensive theory of temporal auditory summation which assumes that loudness is directly proportional to neural activity within the auditory system (Zwislocki, 1960, 1969). These pioneering endeavors led him to determine the role of the slope of the loudness function in temporal summation and its extension to central masking, to assess the relation between the acoustic-reflex growth function and loudness, and more generally, to search for the key to an understanding of the form of the sensation-magnitude functions in all sensory systems (Zwislocki, 1965, 1972, 1973, 1974). Underlying much of Zwislocki’s loudness work is the assumption that Stevens’ power law (1953, 1975), based primarily on direct magnitude-scaling procedures, is determined by the stimulus transformation to a neural loudness code.
One way to ascertain if the outcome of direct magnitude scaling is related to the output of the auditory system is to measure the growth of loudness in cochlear pathology. Cochlear pathology not only produces an elevated threshold, but in the region of impaired hearing it markedly alters the overall shape of the loudness function. This well-documented phenomenon known as loudness recruitment is usually measured by loudness matching (e.g., Fowler, 1928, 1936; Hallpike, 1967; Miskolczy-Fodor, 1960; Reger, 1936).
Although an overall description of the loudness-intensity relation can be derived from measured loudness levels, these indirect loudness determinations cannot disclose the actual rate of growth or shape of the loudness functions (Hellman, 1976; Hellman & Zwislocki, 1968), nor can they provide information about the growth of loudness in bilaterally symmetrical impaired hearing (e.g., Marshall, 1981). Thus, despite their significant theoretical and practical ramifications, loudness-growth data spanning the dynamic range of hearing are seldom obtained for the vast majority of the hearing-impaired population. To help solve this thorny but important problem, several recent studies have demonstrated that direct magnitude scaling advocated by Stevens (1959a, 1975) for the measurement of loudness in auditory pathology can be applied to individuals and groups with cochlear-impaired hearing (Hellman, 1988; Hellman & Meiselman, 1990, 1992, 1993). This chapter presents additional evidence confirming the validity of magnitude scaling for measuring loudness in cochlear impairment. In the first section, equal-sensation functions derived from magnitude scaling are shown to be consistent with the results of intramodality matching. The second section shows that the rate of loudness growth is dependent on threshold sensitivity in the region of impaired hearing but not in the region of normal hearing. Finally, the third section shows that Zwislocki’s (1965) generalized loudness equation gives a good account of the growth of loudness in cochlear impairment. The results are compared to those predicted by the modified power law in the form L = K (P2 − P02)θ (Ekman, 1956; Luce, 1959; Stevens, 1959b) and to predictions by the alternative form L = K (P − P0) subsequently introduced to describe the sensation-magnitude functions in quiet (Zwislocki & Hellman, 1960) and in noise (Lochner & Burger, 1961).

EQUAL-SENSATION FUNCTIONS IN COCHLEAR-IMPAIRED HEARING

Under conditions of minimal experimental constraints and biases Hellman and Zwislocki (1961, 1963, 1964, 1968) showed, in agreement with Stevens (1959a, 1966), that a similar underlying behavior was involved in loudness balances and magnitude scaling, namely matching. The coincidence of equal-sensation matches derived from magnitude scaling with those obtained from direct loudness matching was accomplished by eliminating the confounding effects imposed by either explicit or implicit reference standards and by averaging the individual raw data without any normalization. On the basis of their initial studies, Hellman and Zwislocki postulated that people have the capacity to pair the perceived magnitudes of numbers to sensation magnitudes on an absolute scale. Hence, they reasoned that the outcome of magnitude-scaling experiments can determine both the slope and absolute position of loudness curves on log-log coordinates. Later experiments corroborated the earlier findings and provided a further demonstration that equal-sensation functions consistent with the results of direct matching procedures can be derived from absolute magnitude scaling (Bolanowski, Zwislocki, & Gescheider, 1991; Collins & Gescheider, 1989; Hellman, 1976; Verrillo, Fraioli, & Smith, 1969; Zwislocki, 1983; Zwislocki & Goodman, 1980). The latter studies were all performed with people who had normal sensory functioning. The present experiments show that the mechanics of absolute scaling also hold for individuals with impaired auditory systems.

Description of Experiments

Equal-sensation functions were generated indirectly from absolute magnitude estimation (AME), absolute magnitude production (AMP), and cross-modality matching (CMM) between loudness and apparent length; they were also generated directly from intramodality matching. Eight listeners with bilateral cochlear impairments of long duration participated. All had clinically normal hearing (≤-dB HL; ANSI, 1969) at one frequency enabling either interfrequency or intrafrequency loudness matches to be performed. Each listener was tested individually in a double-walled soundproof booth. Listening was monaural through a TDH-49 earphone mounted in an MX-41/AR cushion. Root-mean-square voltages to the earphone were measured daily with a Fluke (8050A) digital voltmeter.
The stimuli were tone bursts that varied in frequency from 500–3,500 Hz and horizontal lines of light displayed one at a time from 35-mm slides. Tone-burst duration was 1 sec, rise-fall time was 10 ms, and the interstimulus interval was 500 ms. The tones were generated by a Krohn-Hite (4141R) oscillator. After appropriate amplification (Crown D-75 amplifier), the levels of the tone were controlled with Hewlett-Packard (350D) attenuators. A Kodak (4600) projector was used to display the lines which were viewed in a dimly lit room through the glass window of the booth.
The experimental protocol for the determination of the sensation-magnitude functions was as follows. First, pure-tone thresholds were measured by the method of limits at two frequencies, one where thresholds were normal, and the second where thresholds were elevated. Next, to increase the stability of the loudness results and to illustrate the concept of an open-ended number scale (Hellman, 1982; Zwislocki, 1983), apparent length was judged by AME for eight previously tested lines with measured projected lengths of 0.52, 1.04, 2.08, 5.2, 10.4, 20.8, 41.6, and 65 cm (Hellman & Meiselman, 1988, 1990). After the judgments of apparent length were completed, loudness was judged by AME in separate sessions at each of the two chosen stimulus frequencies. Loudness judgments were obtained at 7 to 11 sensation levels (SL) spanning the dynamic range of hearing from 4-dB SL to the SL corresponding to the maximal output of the equipment at a sound-pressure level (SPL) of 110 dB. Within a session, AME was followed by AMP (for rationale, see Hellman & Zwislocki, 1963) and by cross-modality matching. A typical stimulus set for AMP consisted of 7 to 11 numbers, and for CMM it consisted of 6 to 7 lines. For AMP the selected stimuli were individually determined from the range of numbers used for AME of loudness; for CMM they were selected from the lines used for AME of apparent length. As in AME of loudness, judgments by AMP and CMM were obtained at each stimulus frequency in a separate session.
The AME procedure was essentially the same for apparent length and loudness. For both tasks, each listener was simply asked to match an appropriate positive number, including decimals and fractions, to the loudness (or length) of the tone (or line), regardless of the number assigned to the previous stimulus. No standard stimulus was prescribed, and no lengthy practice sessions were required. Moreover, all judgments were self-paced; that is, the listeners were permitted to hear a tone (or see a line) more than once before making a response. Order biases arising from the preceding trial are effectively reduced under these conditions (Hellman, 1976). The final averaging was obtained without normalization of the raw data. Geometric means of the second and third judgments provided the estimate of apparent length and of loudness at each stimulus level.
Similarly, the judgments of AMP of loudness and of CMM were obtained without a designated standard. For both AMP of loudness and CMM the levels of the tone were adjusted by the listener until the loudness appeared equal to the perceived magnitude of the assigned number or line. Three adjustments to each stimulus were obtained by a bracketing procedure. The adjustments were made by turning an unmarked black knob attached to a sone potentiometer (60-dB range) external to the booth. To ensure that the listener’s settings remained within the middle of the potentiometer’s range, the experimenter controlled the input SPL to the potentiometer with a supplementary Hewlett-Packard (350D) attenuator. Just as in AME, the final averages were obtained without normalization of the raw data. Decibel averages of the second and third adjustments provided the average level matched in loudness to each assigned stimulus number or line.
Intramodality matches were obtained in a third listening session with the same apparatus and adjustment procedure used for AMP and CMM. However, rather than match loudness to assigned numbers or lines, six listeners performed intrafrequency matches and two performed interfrequency matches at 6 to 10 levels. The intrafrequency matc...

Table of contents

  1. Cover
  2. Half Title
  3. Title Page
  4. Copyright Page
  5. Dedication
  6. Table of Contents
  7. Preface
  8. Acknowledgments
  9. Letter from Chancellor Kenneth A. Shaw, Syracuse University
  10. Tribute
  11. List of Contributors
  12. Introduction
  13. 1 Can Magnitude Scaling Reveal the Growth of Loudness in Cochlear Impairment?
  14. 2 Control of Rhythmic Firing in Aplysia Neuron R15: A Calcium Riddle
  15. 3 Adaptation and Dynamic Responses in the Auditory Periphery
  16. 4 Intensity Coding and Circadian Rhythms in the Limulus Lateral Eye
  17. 5 The Influence of Long-Range Spatial Interactions on Human Contrast Perception
  18. 6 Cochlear Potentials in Quiet-Aged Gerbils: Does the Aging Cochlea Need a Jump Start?
  19. 7 Photoreceptors, Black Smokers, and Seasonal Affective Disorder: Evidence for Photostasis
  20. 8 And Now, for Our Two Senses
  21. 9 Interaural Temporal Coding of Complex High-Frequency Sounds: A Transformation in the Inferior Colliculus?
  22. 10 Differential Abilities to Extract Sound-Envelope Information by Auditory Nerve and Cochlear Nucleus Neurons
  23. 11 Representing the Surface Texture of Grooved Plates Using Single-Channel, Electrocutaneous Stimulation
  24. 12 Loudness Evaluation by Subjects and by a Loudness Meter
  25. 13 What is Absolute About Absolute Magnitude Estimation?
  26. 14 Involvement of Different Isoforms of Actin in Outer Hair-Cell Motility
  27. 15 Physiology and Functional Implications of a Unique Vertebrate Visual System
  28. 16 Process and Mechanism: Mechanoreceptors in the Mouth as the Primary Modulators of Rhythmic Behavior in Feeding?
  29. 17 The Effects of Aging on the Sense of Touch
  30. 18 Does Efferent Input Improve the Detection of Tones in Monaural Noise?
  31. Author Index
  32. Subject Index