Autism
eBook - ePub

Autism

The Movement Sensing Perspective

  1. 386 pages
  2. English
  3. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  4. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Autism

The Movement Sensing Perspective

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

Autism: The Movement Sensing Perspective is the result of a collaborative effort by parents, therapists, clinicians, and researchers from all disciplines in science including physics, engineering, and applied mathematics. This book poses questions regarding the current conceptualization and approach to the study of autism, providing an alternative unifying data-driven framework grounded in physiological factors.

This book reaches beyond subjective descriptions of autistic phenomena and embraces a new era of objective measurements, analyses, and statistical inferences. The authors harness activities from the nervous systems across the brain and body (often in tandem), and introduce a platform for the comprehensive personalized phenotyping of individuals with autism. The impact of this approach is discussed to advance the development of tailored treatments options, enhance the ability to longitudinally track symptomatology, and to fundamentally empower affected individuals and their families. This book encompasses a new era for autism research and treatments, and our continuous effort to collectively empower and embrace the autistic community.

Frequently asked questions

Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Yes, you can access Autism by Elizabeth B. Torres, Caroline Whyatt in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Medicine & Neurology. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Publisher
CRC Press
Year
2017
ISBN
9781315355245
Edition
1
Subtopic
Neurology
Section II
Basic Research
Movement as a Social Model
4 Dissecting a Social Encounter from Three Different Perspectives
Elizabeth B. Torres
CONTENTS
Introduction
Dissecting a Social Encounter through the Eyes of Different Research Areas
Behaviorist Account from a Psychological Perspective
Physiologist Account
Computational Neuroscientist Account
Guessing Mental States of the Other Party Is Hard and Highly Subjective
Integrating All Three Accounts to Explore Deeper Layers of Detail
References
Chapter 2 closed with the question, is it time for a new model of autism spectrum disorder (ASD)? The psychological and psychiatric constructs defining ASD today describe issues with social interactions in very narrow ways. The methods of inquiry about social and cognitive issues are more an art than a science. They have turned into a stumbling block in scientific advancement, preventing progress toward the discovery of possible causes linking early sensory-motor issues in neurodevelopment with differences in social exchange that emerge later in life. This chapter uses a seemingly simple social encounter to illustrate how, by adopting different perspectives, one can better appreciate and objectively quantify the complexity of the social dyad. Using the hypothetical accounts of a behaviorist, a physiologist, and a computational neuroscientist as they each describe the same encounter, we show that indeed there is more than meets the eye.
INTRODUCTION
Our physical bodies are in constant motion from conception. Even when we are seemingly at rest, our heart is beating, our lungs breathing, and our physiological systems are processing all sorts of electrochemical reactions involving, among other measurable biophysical processes, the transduction and transmission of information across many layers of our nervous systems. And yet, at rest all these motions occur largely without our awareness. We do not see them, and as such, we do not describe them as part of our movements; we do not associate them with what we more generally call behavior. The continuous stream of minute fluctuations in our subtle inner motions, together with our overt actions, may have a profound impact on how we behave, socially or otherwise. Certainly, if you have a stomachache caused by a bad chemical reaction from some spoiled food you ate, you would not be talking about poetry or concocting some clever joke to amuse a crowd of friends. Most likely, you just rather be left alone until it passes.
It must be terrible to not have that option, for example, as when having the physiology of your nervous systems in some continuous state of disarray that confines you to a lonesome existence in such a way you do not even realize, while others around you—perhaps unintentionally—interpret it as your being “antisocial” or having “low empathy.” Conceivably, if they knew of your actual physiological state of disarray, they would try to assist you. But how would they know that your physiological systems are out of whack? They cannot see that in any way, unless they wore some kind of “special glasses” allowing them to see beyond what their naked eyes can naturally capture.
The minute fluctuations that occur in the motions that are sensed throughout the nervous systems can be measured with contemporary instrumentation and provide new lenses into subtle nervous systems’ phenomena. We have coined the waveform representing these fluctuations in biophysical rhythms micromovements (Figure 4.1 and refer back to Chapter 1), as we extract them from bodily and mental rhythms and turn them into quantifiable data output by the nervous systems of the person. Paired with proper analytics, the micromovements can provide a new kind of special glasses to see beyond the obvious and inform us of central nervous system (CNS) and peripheral nervous system (PNS) interactions. To illustrate their use, let us first walk through a simple social situation as described by different (hypothetical) researchers, and then examine some social dyadic interactions using the micromovement perspective.
image
FIGURE 4.1 Extraction of micromovements is possible across different biophysical signals from physiological states registered in different body parts using wearable sensors. (a) Taxonomy of control levels involving different layers of micromovements mapping to different stochastic signatures. (b) Division of labor from early infancy during neurodevelopment impact different somatic motor networks in the face- and body-relevant dimensions of the social axes. (Reproduced with permission from Torres, E. B. et al., Front. Pediatr. 4:121, 2016.)
DISSECTING A SOCIAL ENCOUNTER THROUGH THE EYES OF DIFFERENT RESEARCH AREAS
Let us attempt to deconstruct the social encounter depicted in Figure 4.2. The encounter in question takes place between two people who may have seen each other once before. Through the length of time they sustain eye contact as they approach each other, they may admit to each other recalling their first fortuitous encounter sometime in the past, or they may right there and then decide to make the eye contact so brief that there is no ambiguity in their unwillingness to proceed with a social exchange. Notice here that this is a deliberate decision, rather than a spontaneously occurring event.
image
FIGURE 4.2 Genesis of a brief social encounter. Two fellows walking toward each other recognize their acquaintance from the distance and try to discern whether the other person is willing to admit to this mutual memory and engage in a brief social exchange, like a salutation and small talk. Body language involving sustained eye contact and facial microexpressions, including a smile, may give away the mutual willingness to stop, shake hands, and say hello. Alternatively, even a brief gesture like turning the body away from the interlocutor’s body midline and avoiding eye contact, perhaps accompanied by spontaneous (flat) facial microexpressions denoting an unwillingness to engage, will determine the fate of this brief encounter.
If their eye contact was sustained long enough to go on with the social exchange, they may further provide mutual evidence for their willingness to proceed by orienting their body toward each other and extending their hands to prompt a handshake—we will safely assume here that this is an acceptable social form of salutation in the culture where these two individuals developed. And finally, they will execute the handshake and say hello to each other, perhaps going on to initiate some social chat.
This is a very hypothetical situation. We shall dissect that social encounter using the lens of a researcher whose area of expertise has to do with social behaviors, another researcher whose area of expertise focuses on the physiology of the nervous systems underlying that social behavior, and yet another view through the lens of a researcher whose area of expertise is modeling the types of sensory-motor integration processes and sensory-motor transformations that may underlie that social behavior through the use of mathematical and computational tools.
Finally, let us examine examples of dyadic social interactions using a research program that integrates all three accounts through an interdisciplinary collaborative approach.
BEHAVIORIST ACCOUNT FROM A PSYCHOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVE
To study this seemingly simple social exchange, the behavioral psychologist will most likely draw a discrete set of events and assign a discrete (ordinal) scale to each event unambiguously detected by the naked eye. The account may go as follows (for example): (1) eye contact, (2) smile, (3) handshake, (4) word exchange (“Hello!”), and (5) initiate chat. Each one of these five events may have a subscale, say from 1 to 10, with 1 signifying poor and 10 excellent, and some other considerations in between. The researcher will go on and collect data on each of these events (1–5) according to each of the numbers coded by hand to “quantify” the overall behavior. The behaviorist may then use traditional statistical tools and analyze the cross-sectional data by averaging scores across large numbers of subjects. This will build a normative data set with the potential to help identify atypical patterns in the future based on what is “normally” expected in such a social encounter (as defined by the inventory). Examples of such approaches abound in the fields of clinical psychology and psychiatry. In fact, these types of structured inventories are commonly used to gather criteria to diagnose disorders that involve social deficits as mental illnesses, as well as to treat them through behavior-reshaping methods or prescribed psychotropic medications (Lord et al. 1989, 2001; Lord et al.; American Psychiatric Association 2013). Notably, none of these clinical inventories ever characterized normative data, so they feature absolute ordinal values of some arbitrary scale. As such, they are not properly standardized.
PHYSIOLOGIST ACCOUNT
The physiologist will set up high-grade instrumentation using a variety of sensors to register signals throughout the nervous systems. Today’s technological advances allow for noninvasive methods of data registration. Using contemporary (e.g., wireless) instrumentation, the physiologist will attempt to continuously register every detail of this exchange. She may track the eye motions to assess the length of time on a millisecond timescale that person-to-person foveation was sustained, quantify the saccades and the smooth pursuit eye motions throughout the exchange, and record (for example) heat activity from the facial muscles using thermal cameras strategically positioned to capture various facial configurations and detect universal signatures describing microexpressions of the face (Ekman and Rosenberg 1997). Then these data will be used to automatically infer possible emotional states. The physiologist may also record neck motions (perhaps with wearable inertial measurement units and wearable electromyographic sensors). Neck position and orientation will reveal trajectories of the head in the body, and eye tracking technology will reveal the position and orientation of the eyes in the head. This information will enable assessment of the system’s use of different frames of references during sensory-motor transformations required in simple goal-directed saccades and hand motions. Simultaneously, bodily rhythms from all movable joints will also provide important information from each individual in the dyad and from their interactions as the multiple degrees of freedom in both participants coarticulate and the synergies dynamically fall in and out of synchrony. As the bodily rhythms fluctuate, so do the speech rhythms that the physiologist can record with a microphone.
The speech, generated by the sensory-motor apparatus from orofacial structures, will provide a rich body of data to—in concert with the face data—further help assess emotional components of the encounter. Likewise, sensors coregistering electrocardiograms and electrodermal activity (EDA) will provide several l...

Table of contents

  1. Title Page
  2. Copyright Page
  3. Table of Contents
  4. Preface
  5. Foreword
  6. Contributors
  7. Section I: The Big Question: Why Study Movement?
  8. Section II: Basic Research: Movement as a Social Model
  9. Section III: Let’s Get the Math Right to Improve Diagnosis, Research, and Treatment Outcomes
  10. Section IV: The Therapeutic Model: Movement as a Percept to Awaken the Mind
  11. Section V: Autism, the Untold Story from the Perspectives of Parents and Self-advocates
  12. Conclusions
  13. Index