An Introduction to Language and Society
eBook - ePub

An Introduction to Language and Society

  1. 316 pages
  2. English
  3. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  4. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

An Introduction to Language and Society

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

In this third edition of the bestselling classic textbook, Martin Montgomery explores the key connections between language and social life. Guiding the student through discussions on child language, accent and dialect, social class and gender, as well as a number of other topics, Montgomery provides a comprehensive and accessible introduction to the function of language in modern society.

This third edition includes:



  • new sections on dialect levelling and estuary English; hip-hop and rapping as anti-language and 'crossing' between Creole, Panjabi and South Asian English
  • new material on the Gulf War and the 'War on Terror'
  • discussions on language in internet usage and new technologies
  • updated examples and references.

With detailed suggestions for further reading and practical work for each chapter, An Introduction to Language and Society is the ideal resource for students and teachers of Communication Studies and Language Studies.

Frequently asked questions

Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Yes, you can access An Introduction to Language and Society by Martin Montgomery in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Languages & Linguistics & Linguistics. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Publisher
Routledge
Year
2013
ISBN
9781136010309
Edition
1

PART ONE
THE DEVELOPMENT
OF LANGUAGE

In the beginning was the word.
(John 1:1)
In the beginning was the deed. The word followed as its phonetic shadow.
(Leon Trotsky*)
____________________
* Trotsky, L. (1925) Literature and Revolution

1THE BEGINNINGS OF LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT

Learning language: the first words

It first becomes obvious that children are actually learning to talk some time between twelve and eighteen months of age when they begin to use single-word utterances of the following type:
juice dada biscuit
there no that
byebye big wassat
hi shoe allgone
car look dirty
hot up ball
Such simple single-word utterances may look like a very primitive and rudimentary communication system. The vocabulary in the early stages is limited in range and its application is restricted to the immediate here and now. Consequently, it is hardly possible for the child to express or give shape to complex relationships, such as:
the conditional (‘if…, then …’);
the temporal (‘before’ and ‘after’);
notions of probability (‘might’, ‘may’, ‘could’, etc.).
But the first one-word utterances are not as simple as they look. For one thing, children use them in a variety of ways. Thus, while the list above includes expressions for salient objects in their immediate environment (items such as ‘shoe’, ‘car’,‘juice’, ‘biscuit’, etc.), it also includes items for opening and closing encounters (‘byebye’, ‘hi’), items for attracting someone's attention (‘dada’) and focusing it in a particular direction (‘look’), items for refusing or resisting a particular course of action (‘no!’) and for commenting on a particular state of affairs (‘dirty’, ‘allgone’). Furthermore, it is not uncommon for the child to have at least one question form (e.g. ‘wassat?’) and to be able to use some of the items to request actions of others (e.g. ‘up’ as meaning ‘lift me up’). And, while some of the items seem only ever to be used for one purpose (e.g. ‘hi’ as a greeting), others can serve a variety of purposes: for example, ‘dada’ can be used as a call, a greeting or a comment (as meaning ‘there's a man in the photograph’); ‘biscuit’ can be used as a request (‘I want a biscuit now’), or as a comment (‘that's a biscuit on the table’), or even as a kind of acknowledgement, as in the following interchange:
ADULT: here's your biscuit
CHILD: biscuit ((takes biscuit))
There is, then, more to the child's first words than might initially appear. They are from the beginning much more than merely names for objects, ways of referring to things around them. Indeed, even such a restricted range of items provides a highly subtle means of engaging with and responding to others. They enable the child to establish and maintain contact with others and they allow for the expression of diverse dispositions such as hunger, interest, curiosity, pleasure, warmth and anger. So, although the range of items that initially comprise the child's vocabulary may be small, the purposes that such items can be made to serve in use are highly varied.
Studies of child language development usually refer to these one-word utterances as ‘holophrases’, implying thereby that these single items carry a broader and more diffuse range of meaning than do their equivalents in the language of adults: basically, children at this stage make single words do the work of the fully constructed sentences they will probably be producing a year or so later.

Some precursors of language development

Language development, however, does not begin suddenly out of the blue with the ‘holophrastic stage’. Children can, for instance, understand and respond in appropriate non-verbal ways to the utterances of others long before they begin to produce holophrases. This is partly because, in the everyday routines of feeding, washing, dressing and play, the infant will be exposed to myriad repetitions of identical linguistic forms in recognizably similar contexts; and so, by the time the child begins to speak, s/he has already begun to grasp the basic outlines of the language system of his/her culture.

The infant cry

The first year of life sees the gradual emergence of very generalized capacities to interact — capacities that precede and provide a basis for the specifically linguistic developments later on. From the moment of birth, for example, infants can impinge dramatically on those around them: they can cry. Indeed, from the first weeks of life they have a repertoire of at least three distinguishable types of cry — the hunger cry, the pain cry, and a cry associated with fatigue, boredom or discomfort — each sounding subtly different from the other. The hunger cry, or ‘basic cry’ as it is sometimes known, is a moderately pitched loud cry that builds into a rhythmic cycle made up of the cry itself followed by a short silence, then an intake of breath followed by another short silence before the next cry resumes the cycle. Repetition of the series through successive cycles gives the cry its rhythmic quality. The discomfort cry or ‘grumble’ is lower in pitch, more variable in volume, though generally quieter than the basic cry. Its rhythm is slower and may be interspersed with grunts and sucking noises. The pain cry is markedly different again, taking the form of an inward gasp followed by a high-pitched, long-drawn-out rising shriek.
These cries are, of course, most important for survival in the first weeks of life. They provide a key resource, perhaps the only resource, open to the infant for signalling crucial physiological states such as hunger or pain; and as such they seem pitched at a level likely to evoke most disturbance in another human. Initially they may well be purely reflex responses to physiological states; but, if matters are ever that simple, they do not remain so for long. For one thing the different forms of cry often shade into one another. A child may begin with a grumble that gradually shifts into a hunger cry, or begin with a pain cry that gradually subsides into the basic cry rhythm. An adult accustomed to looking after a particular child or children may know instantly what a specific cry indicates, but just as often there is some indeterminacy and it requires the exercise of several kinds of awareness simultaneously to interpret what a particular instance of crying is all about. Thus, any one cry has to be gauged against the others that may have preceded it. It has also to be interpreted in the context of the recurring cycle of child care (Is it long since the last feed? How long since the last nappy change? etc.). And finally, any particular cry is interpreted in the light of any known special circumstances affecting the child; for instance, a tendency to get fretful after a wakeful period late in the day. Basically, those involved in the care of the child find themselves discriminating between the different noises that the child makes, and arriving at different interpretations of them.
The noises themselves, of course, do not as yet amount to fully fledged acts of communication. They seem to be reflex responses to physiological states rather than acts performed with the deliberate intention of conveying a specific meaning to a recipient. Even so, there is some evidence to suggest that infants are highly responsive to the human voice, and that a ‘grumbling’ infant, for example, will often break off from crying if spoken to by an adult. Indeed, it is not long before those who look after the child detect what seems best described as a ‘fake’ cry — one that is related to no clear physiological need but is apparently designed to provoke attention from the adult. This marks the crucial beginnings of a growing displacement of the child's vocalizations: they are no longer so firmly entrenched in bodily needs but become available for use as a social and interactive resource.

Further developments in vocalization

The child, of course, has still many steps to take before developing the fully articulated powers of speech. There will be a phase of ‘cooing’ around three months and this gives way in turn to a period of ‘babbling’ from around six months onwards. Vocalization now begins to take on features of the speech of the surrounding community, especially in terms of the tone of voice and intonation with its attendant stress and rhythm patterns. The babbling can sound at times almost conversational but it is hard to detect anything meaningful in its perform...

Table of contents

  1. Cover
  2. Half Title
  3. Full Title
  4. Copyright
  5. Dedication
  6. Contents
  7. Preface to the second edition
  8. Preface to the third edition
  9. Acknowledgements
  10. Transcription conventions
  11. Introduction Background sources and further reading
  12. PART ONE THE DEVELOPMENT OF LANGUAGE
  13. PART TWO LINGUISTIC DIVERSITY AND THE SPEECH COMMUNITY
  14. PART THREE LANGUAGE AND SOCIAL INTERACTION
  15. PART FOUR LANGUAGE AND REPRESENTATION
  16. References
  17. Index