Speaking With Style (RLE Linguistics C: Applied Linguistics)
eBook - ePub

Speaking With Style (RLE Linguistics C: Applied Linguistics)

The Sociolinguistics Skills of Children

  1. 216 pages
  2. English
  3. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  4. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Speaking With Style (RLE Linguistics C: Applied Linguistics)

The Sociolinguistics Skills of Children

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

In acquiring communicative competence, children must learn to speak not only grammatically but also appropriately. Although rules for appropriate language use may vary from culture to culture, they are usually sensitive across languages to many of the same factors, including the context and the topic of the discourse, and the sex, age, familiarity and relative status of the speaker and the listener. There is available detailed evidence of the ways in which adults consistently modify their speech to foreigners, of phonological, syntactic, and lexical markings of language in professional settings, and of differences in men's and women's speech that are tied to their roles in society.

This book examines young children's knowledge of the sociolinguistic rules that govern appropriate language use, exploring (i) the repertoire of registers (ie speech varieties) that young children possess; (ii) the linguistic devices that they use to mark distinct registers; (iii) the way their skill in using these registers develops.

Frequently asked questions

Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Yes, you can access Speaking With Style (RLE Linguistics C: Applied Linguistics) by Elaine Andersen in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Languages & Linguistics & Linguistics. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Publisher
Routledge
Year
2014
ISBN
9781317932116
Edition
1
Chapter one
Image
Introduction
Image
The more we study speech in natural settings, the more we find systematic variation within every speaker, reflecting who he is addressing, where he is, what the social event may be, the topic of discussion, and the social relations he communicates by speaking. The regularities in these features of speech make them as amenable to analysis as the abstracted rules called grammars. Competence in speaking includes the ability to use appropriate speech for the circumstance and when deviating from the normal to convey what is intended. It would be an incompetent speaker who used babytalk to everyone or randomly interspersed sentences in babytalk or in a second language regardless of circumstance. It would be equally incompetent to use formal style in all situations and to all addresses in a society allowing for a broader range of variation.
(Ervin-Tripp 1973a: 268)
In acquiring full communicative competence, children must learn to speak not only grammatically, but also appropriately (Hymes 1972). At some time during acquisition they must learn a variety of sociolinguistic and social interactional rules which govern appropriate language use. Though the language addressed to 2-year-olds may be highly specialized, by the time children reach age 4 or 5, they have experienced diverse speech settings: they go to the doctor, to preschool, to birthday parties, to the grocery store. They participate in a variety of speech situations, with people who differ in age, sex, status and familiarity, and whose speech will therefore vary in a number of systematic ways.
Are young children aware of these sociolinguistic and social interactional differences? What do they know about the appropriateness of linguistic forms used to indicate particular situations and particular roles and relationships?
Unfortunately, there has been relatively little investigation of these questions. Until the last decade or so, most research on children’s language was of a psycholinguistic nature, centred on one or more aspects of phonological, morphosyntactic, or semantic development. This focus was in large part due to the concept of linguistic competence proposed by Chomsky (1959, 1965) which provided the major impetus for studies of first language acquisition.
The notion of competence and language acquisition
In Chomsky’s view, a speaker’s linguistic competence is the purely grammatical knowledge of an ideal speaker-hearer that allows him to produce the infinitely large set of sentences that constitutes his language. This knowledge is seen largely as an innate biological function of mind, and the importance of child language research is therefore to document the emergence of innately determined grammatical structures in order to test the linguistic theory about the nature of competence. A major distinction is drawn between competence and performance, i.e. language behaviour, with the latter said to be determined by ‘non-linguistic’ factors such as physiological and psychological factors (e.g. fatigue) impacting actual production, as well as speakers’ attitudes and beliefs about the world, and the social conventions of the speech community of which one is a member. With this framework, child language research in the 1960s was devoted almost exclusively to studies of grammar, testing, for example, whether a transformational model could account for the acquisition of question forms, of passives, or of various other syntactic structures. Toward the end of the 1960s, as many of these accounts proved unsatisfactory, there was a semantic revolution of sorts both in child language study, and in linguistic theory more generally (Fillmore 1968; Lakoff 1970; McCawley 1971). Researchers in acquisition came to realize that it was impossible to evaluate children’s knowledge without examining the context in which they use their early language and the kinds of semantic relations they encode (Bloom 1970). Common to the syntax-focused work of the 1960s and the semantic-based work of the 1970s was a search for universals in the acquisition of structure, assuming an innate basis to language (be it purely linguistic as in the first case, or more generally cognitive, as in the second), which would result in all normal children passing through the same stages in the acquisition of competence in the same order.
Since that time, however, a growing number of researchers in sociolinguistics and psycholinguistics have become dissatisfied with a monolithic, idealized notion of competence. When linguists like Labov (1966) and Halliday (1970) began to pay greater attention to intra-language variation, their proposals required a broadening of Chomsky’s view of what needed to be accounted for in language acquisition. Campbell and Wales (1970), for example, proposed that competence should be extended to include the native speaker’s capacity to produce or understand utterances appropriate to the verbal and situational context. This expanded notion of competence, then, subsumed part of what previous notions allocated to the domain of performance: knowledge of the ways in which social conventions and the speaker’s attitudes and beliefs about the world systematically impact language structure. The change of emphasis in linguistic theory was paralleled by a shift in focus in studies of acquisition. Since children acquiring language must obviously learn more than grammatical rules and vocabulary alone, other aspects of their communicative competence are worthy of attention:
We have then to account for the fact that a normal child acquires knowledge of sentences, not only as grammatical, but also as appropriate. He or she acquires competence as to when to speak, when not, and as to what to talk about with whom, when, where, in what manner. In short, a child becomes able to accomplish a repertoire of speech acts, to take part in speech events, and to evaluate their accomplishment by others.
(Hymes 1972: 277)
Although rules for appropriate language use may vary from culture to culture, they are usually sensitive across languages to many of the same factors, including the context and topic of discourse, and the sex, age, and status of the people speaking. In most languages, for instance, adults speak in one way to young children, in another to older children, and in yet another to fellow adults (Fernald 1984; Ferguson 1977; Andersen 1975); doctors address their patients in one way and consulting physicians or friends in another (Shuy 1976); students ‘change their style’ when they leave a peer group in the corridor to join an academic discussion in the classroom (De Stefano 1972a, 1972b; Houston 1969a, 1970); native speakers consistently modify their speech when addressing foreigners (Ferguson 1975); and men and women exhibit manners of speech which may differ to a greater or lesser degree, dependent in part on their roles in society (Thorne, Kramerae and Henley 1983; Thorne and Henley 1975; Keenan 1974; Lakoff 1973).
These stylistic differences, which are referred to as register variation (see Chapter 2), are often very subtle. Indeed, when a foreigner has attained near-native ability in a second language, it is often along the dimension of appropriateness that his speech reveals his incomplete knowledge: The choice of items from the wrong register, and the mixing of items from different registers, are among the most frequent mistakes made by non-native speakers of a language’ (Halliday et al. 1970: 150). For example, such a speaker may use a colloquial expression in too formal a situation, or a female non-native speaker may use a form considered especially ‘masculine’ in a given culture. Thus, there are a large number of social skills in communication that children must acquire before they can be said to have mastered the use of their native language.
Lakoff (1973) and others (see Piaget 1970, on the egocentric nature of children’s language) have suggested that children aged 4 or 5 are unaware of many rules of socio-cultural appropriateness, but that by age 10 or so they can use differentiated ‘styles’, or registers of speech. But a number of other studies have indicated that this is an underestimate of the child’s abilities – that instead children learn to make some context-sensitive stylistic adjustments in their speech at a much earlier age (Ervin-Tripp and Gordon 1986; Sachs and Devin 1976; Andersen and Johnson 1973; Shatz and Gelman 1973). These studies, however, either have been quite limited in focus or have looked at only a few children. They leave unanswered many questions of exactly how and when during acquisition children learn to use their language appropriately.
The present study
The study which I will present in this book examines young children’s knowledge of the sociolinguistic rules that govern appropriate language use, exploring (i) the repertoire of registers (i.e., situationally determined language varieties) that young children possess; (ii) the linguistic devices that they use to mark distinct registers; and (iii) the way their skill in using these registers develops. This research differs from other studies of the development of sociolinguistic skills in the range of linguistic markers and social dimensions that are explored. It does so by employing a research methodology which I have called ‘controlled improvisation’ which allows the investigator to elicit a very rich corpus of data, comparable across children, in a time- and energy-efficient manner. This method will be discussed in Chapter 4. First, however, I will turn in the next chapter to a brief survey of the sociolinguistic studies of register variation in adult populations, followed in Chapter 3 by a selected overview of recent research in ‘developmental sociolinguistics’. Chapters, 5, 6, and 7 explore different linguistic devices used by young children to mark distinct registers, looking first at ‘Discourse structure and content’, then at ‘Variation in utterance form and function’, and finally at the ‘Phonological, lexical, and morphological markings of register’. Chapter 8 provides a brief summary and conclusions about the relevance of these findings, not only to a general theory of language acquisition, but also to theories of social and cognitive development.
Chapter two
Image
The study of register variation
Image
Every human being is a bundle of institutionalized roles. He has to play many parts, and unless he knows his lines as well as his role he is no use in the play.
(Firth, quoted in Verma 1969: 293)
The notion of ‘register’
Doctors from Atlanta speak differently from doctors from Brooklyn; they come from different regions of the United States. Salespeople at Saks in New York speak differently from salespeople at Gimbels in that same city; they come from different socio-economic classes (Labov 1966). The speech of grandparents varies in a number of systematic ways from the speech of their grandchildren; they are from different generations. The area of linguistics which traditionally has been concerned with such differences is called dialectology: regional and ‘temporal’ dialects have long been a topic of study for linguists (Bloomfield 1933); social dialects have also been of long-standing interest (Labov 1972(a); Bernstein 1960; Sweet 1928).
There is, however, another dimension along which one can arrange language varieties. While dialects vary in relation to characteristics of users (i.e. where they are from, their social class), there are other varieties within any language which are distinguished by the circumstances of their use. The use of the term ‘register’ to describe this form of variation was introduced by Reid (1956) who first analysed the phenomenon in the context of bilingualism. (Other terms – most notably ‘speech style’, ‘variety’, and ‘code’ – have also been used, but ‘register’ seems preferable to them, largely because, unlike the others, it has not already been applied with other meanings to such fields as literature, dialectology, etc.1) Reid pointed out that in many bilingual or multilingual communities, given languages serve discrete functions. Language x, for example, may be used in the classroom, in the newspaper, in government, etc., while language y is used in the market-place or at home. Ferguson (1959) observed an analogous situation (which he labelled ‘diglossia’) in a number of monolingual communities, where, instead of languages varying with function, speakers simply use different forms of the same language – e.g., classical Arabic in the classroom and colloquial Arabic in the student lounge.
Although the registers of a given language may not differ from one another as greatly as classical Arabic does from colloquial Arabic, they are linguistically distinct varieties of speech. Each register displays a systematic language patterning used in a specific type of situation; each represents a well-established convention within a language community. As Verma points out: ‘[Registers] cut across dialect varieties and may be used for specific purposes by all the speakers/writers of a language’ (Verma 1969: 294).
Registers, then, are far from being marginal aspects of a language; rather, they determine how language is used in varying situations. The range of registers which exists in a community covers the total range of our language activity (Halliday et al. 1964: 89).
Though registers are shared by a number of speakers, speakers differ as to whether their control of these registers is active or only passive. Most of us recognize and respond to many registers that we never use, an example being the language of sermons. The range of registers controlled by a given individual, and his degree of control over each, presumably reflect that individual’s language experience. The registers people have in their active repertoires, then, are probably only a subset of those available, a...

Table of contents

  1. Cover
  2. Half Title
  3. Title Page
  4. Copyright Page
  5. Original Title Page
  6. Original Copyright Page
  7. Dedication
  8. Table of Contents
  9. Preface
  10. 1. Introduction
  11. 2. The study of register variation
  12. 3. An overview of developmental sociolinguistics
  13. 4. Methodological considerations
  14. 5. Discourse structure and content
  15. 6. Variation in utterance form and function
  16. 7. Phonological, lexical and morphological markings of register
  17. 8. Conclusions
  18. Bibliography
  19. Index