Deterrence and Crime Prevention
eBook - ePub

Deterrence and Crime Prevention

Reconsidering the Prospect of Sanction

  1. 208 pages
  2. English
  3. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  4. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Deterrence and Crime Prevention

Reconsidering the Prospect of Sanction

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

Deterrence is at the heart of the preventive aspiration of criminal justice. Deterrence, whether through preventive patrol by police officers or stiff prison sentences for violent offenders, is the principal mechanism through which the central feature of criminal justice, the exercise of state authority, works – it is hoped -- to diminish offending and enhance public safety. And however well we think deterrence works, it clearly often does not work nearly as well as we would like – and often at very great cost.

Drawing on a wide range of scholarly literatures and real-world experience, Kennedy argues that we should reframe the ways in which we think about and produce deterrence. He argues that many of the ways in which we seek to deter crime in fact facilitate offending; that simple steps such as providing clear information to offenders could transform deterrence; that communities may be far more effective than legal authorities in deterring crime; that apparently minor sanctions can deter more effectively than draconian ones; that groups, rather than individual offenders, should often be the focus of deterrence; that existing legal tools can be used in unusual but greatly more effective ways; that even serious offenders can be reached through deliberate moral engagement; and that authorities, communities, and offenders – no matter how divided – share and can occupy hidden common ground.

The result is a sophisticated but ultimately common-sense and profoundly hopeful case that we can and should use new deterrence strategies to address some of our most important crime problems. Drawing on and expanding on the lessons of groundbreaking real-world work like Boston's Operation Ceasefire – credited with the "Boston Miracle" of the 1990s – "Deterrence and Crime Prevention" is required reading for scholars, law enforcement practitioners, and all with an interest in public safety and the health of communities.

Frequently asked questions

Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Yes, you can access Deterrence and Crime Prevention by David M. Kennedy in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Business & Business General. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Publisher
Routledge
Year
2012
ISBN
9781135976309
Edition
1

1 Does deterrence work?

We begin with the most basic question: Does deterrence work? Strong statements are often made in the negative. “Deterrence is a principal with much immediate appeal,” wrote England's Home Office in 1990,
but much crime is committed on impulse […] and it is committed by offenders who live from moment to moment […] It is unrealistic to construct sentencing arrangements on the assumption that most offenders will weigh up the possibilities in advance and base their conduct on rational calculation.1
In fact, the world is soaked in deterrence, something that easily escapes notice because of its utter ordinariness. The class of people who persistently put their hands on hot stoves, cross the street without looking, and steal cars in front of police officers is very small. The class of New York City neighborhoods in which drivers routinely ignore alternate-side parking regulations (and, in parallel, fail to respond adroitly to the occasional lifting of those regulations) is precisely nil, a finding confirmed by the author's extensive participant-observer research.
The expectation of official action clearly does not deter all offending, but it equally clearly deters a lot of offending, and when it is altered or removed, people's behavior changes. There is an abundance of examples of such influence and adaptation, much of it fascinating. Many prosecutors, for instance, have office policies setting nonstatutory minima for drug prosecutions, with arrests for less than a set quantity generally being dismissed or charged as possession rather than trafficking. The experience of police in those jurisdictions is that drug offenders soon learn those minima and scrupulously stay under them, carrying, for example, exactly twenty-seven pieces of crack rather than the informal and arbitrary but well-understood twenty-eight.2 Ethnographic research into illicit gun markets in Chicago showed that gangs restrict their members from carrying and using guns, one reason being that police attention following gun violence interferes with money-making crime such as drug-dealing.4 Motorcycle gang members in the Midwest were subject, as convicted felons, to substantial federal penalties for carrying guns; they took, instead, to carrying nineteenth-century percussion firearms exempted under an obscure “curios and relics” provision in the governing criminal statute.5
Ordinary experience validates the common sense of deterrence—at least with some people, with respect to some offenses, some of the time—and the idea that existing criminal-justice activities are in fact creating deterrence. When police departments go on strikes, bad things tend to happen.6 A great deal of research, much of which this book will draw on, ends up pointing to much the same conclusion. “My assessment,” writes Philip Cook, one of our most thoughtful and thorough students on the subject, “is that the criminal justice system, ineffective though it may seem in many areas, has an overall crime deterrent effect of great magnitude.”7
The equally obvious frequent failure of deterrence is not probative that the general idea is mistaken or that sanction structures aimed at deterrence are having no impact. Part of the problem here is the frequent commission of the prison warden's fallacy: that the prisons are filled with men manifestly not deterred by legal threats is not evidence that other men were not deterred. Strong claims that deterrence is ineffective are often in fact arguments that the limits of deterrence have been reached and that additional enforcement and penalties will not produce additional deterrent impact. That may be correct, but it is not evidence that deterrence as such does not work. It does raise the considerably more interesting question of whether deterrence can be extended beyond those limits, and if so how: a question that will consume most of this book.
Assessments of particular policy interventions also support the operation of deterrence: in, again, given contexts. There are very few controlled experiments in deterrence, but quasi-experiments and more ordinary evaluations frequently show impact. In a set of controlled trials, mandatory arrest for domestic violence reduced recidivism in a number of cities;8 in another controlled trial, increased police presence in high-crime areas reduced crime in those areas.9 A controlled trial that applied largely deterrent tactics reduced crime in drug-trafficking hot spots.10 Intensive police patrol reduced robberies in New York City subways.11 The prospect of greatly enhanced drunk-driving enforcement in the United Kingdom reduced both drunk driving and related accidents.12 Steps to increase the apprehension of airline hijackers in the early 1970s all but ended hijacking, not because of actual apprehensions but because the likelihood of being detected rose to such a prohibitive level that people stopped trying.13 A mandatory one-year sentence for the illegal possession of a firearm reduced gun-carrying and gun crime in Massachusetts.14 Mandatory sentences for gun offenses reduced violent crime in a pool of six cities.15 A study of arrest ratios in 171 cities found that increased enforcement deterred street crime.16 Crackdowns on street dealing dried up drug markets in Lynn, Massachusetts.17 The announcement that a vigorous and established powder cocaine market in Houston would be, on a date certain, the focus of an interagency law enforcement crackdown dried up the market before the crackdown could actually be implemented.18 Intensive directed patrol aimed at identified offenders reduced homicide and gun crime in Indianapolis, Indiana.19 The record thus shows that deterrence is not just operating, it can be deliberately created in at least some settings and against at least some forms of offending.
A long tradition of “perceptual deterrence” social-science research has gone at the deterrence question in a different way.20 This research sought to unpack—using individual-level cross-sectional survey and scenario studies, and panel survey studies—how and why people reacted to what they thought, or were told, would happen if they offended; why they did, or did not, care about that; and how it shaped their subsequent behavior. This research, too, is generally supportive of the idea that deterrence is operating. While varied, the cross-sectional and scenario studies tended to find that higher perceived risks of sanction were associated with lack of offending or of likelihood to offend. Waldo and Chiricos, for example, found that college students who thought that they would be arrested and convicted for marijuana use and theft were less likely to say that they would commit those offenses.21 In general, the certainty and, to a lesser extent, the swiftness of sanction mattered more than the severity of sanction, to the extent that many researchers concluded that severity was all but or in fact irrelevant.
A core theme in deterrence research has to do with what ultimately came to be called the “experiential effect”: the observation that, as time passes, many people come to lower their estimates of the risks of offending. The debate began in interpreting survey research showing that low risk estimates were associated with higher levels of offending. The initial conclusion was that low estimates of risk led to offending, with associated ideas of criminogenic personality types resistant to deterrence.22 Subsequent thinking shifted to the idea that, instead, offenders were simply more informed and realistic: that “most people (young and old) have over pessimistic estimates of their ability to get away with breaking the law […] but that when they participate in illegal acts they soon discover that they can commit them with relative impunity.”23 According to this idea, as offenders commit crimes and escape sanction, or see others do so, they adjust their risk estimates downward. This is not a problem with the idea of deterrence, or with some sort or sorts of undeterrable personality, but with the objective characteristics of the sanction regime supposed to generate deterrence. Experience breaches the “shell of illusion” that one can't get away with offending.24
This set of issues led to perceptual deterrence research using panel studies that allowed assessment of how attitudes toward risk evolved over time on the basis of concrete experience with offending and sanction, and to other research designed to get at these issues. This research generally supported both the idea that deterrence was operating and that its effects could be vitiated when people thought that their objective risks were low.25 Much of the research supporting this conclusion was conducted outside populations of serious offenders and assessed attitudes by subjects such as college students to low-level offenses, but the basic proposition—people learn from experience that they face rather low risks and act accordingly—may plausibly be generalized. The rare studies of these dynamics with more or less serious offenders bore them out in that population. Horney and Marshall surveyed incarcerated felons and found that those with more experience of offending had lower risk estimates, and that the actual rate of prior arrests moved those perceptions: For most crimes in the survey, those offenders who had been arrested relatively often, compared to the number of crimes they had committed, felt more at risk.26 Lochner, analyzing data in large national surveys, found much the same dynamic amongst youth offenders.27 This turned out to be one of the main reasons that some perceptual research turned up an absence of deterrence effects: Subjects knew perfectly well that risks were low and maintained that knowledge even after being sanctioned. Though they also often updated their risk assessments, they remained low enough to justify further offending. As we shall see, this can be true even for extremely serious crimes.
So, deterrence effects are there to be found. The strong case—“deterrence does not work”—is clearly false. Deterrence effects are also, often, found to be absent. The weaker case—that deterrence does not work very well or does not work where we would particularly like it to, or is not worth the various costs of producing it—is still a live option. Even those who believe in deterrence can be realistically dubious about its actual impact and the prospect for enhancing that impact. “Thus the problem faced by those responsible for the everyday enforcement of law in democratic societies is that deterrent effects are apparently modest [and] certainty of punishment is low under many circumstances,” wrote the Committee to Review Research on Police Policy and Practices of the National Academy of Sciences. “As a consequence, deterrence strategies alone are unlikely to be a sufficient basis for an effective system of social regulation.”28
As on the positive side, the most telling evidence is ordinary and everyday: In the face of a great deal of punishment, offending continues. The death penalty manifestly fails to deter a good many murderers. Recidivism is very high amongst offenders who have had prior arrests and convictions and who thus have good reason to fear sanctions: A 2002 Bureau of Justice Statistics study showed that 67.5 percent of those released from US prisons in 1994 were rearrested for a new crime within three years.29 Striking increases in sanctions, as for drug crimes in the mid-1980s, failed to curb widespread drug epidemics. Explicit official promises of particular sanctions for particular crimes, as with stiff federal mandatory sentences for gun carrying, fail to reduce gun homicide.30 Research with actual criminals frequently reveals that the mechanisms designed to produce deterrence are not operating remotely as intended. Doob and Webster report on a study based on interviews with sixty experienced, and presently incarcerated, burglars and armed robbers. “The respondents were blunt in reporting that neither they nor other thieves whom they knew considered legal consequences when planning crimes. Thoughts about getting caught were put out of their minds.”31 This is devastating to the deterrence project: If offenders not only do not fear consequences but willfully refuse to even consider them, we simply cannot reach them.
All this cannot be taken to mean that deterrence is not operating, in principle or in fact. Offending might be even higher than it is in the absence of these sanctions; higher sanctions might yet work. But it is at least evidence that existing deterrence regimes, some involving policies on which we rely heavily, including some involving very stiff sanctions, are not working as intended. This, again, accords with the common sense. We are reluctant to think that consequences do not matter, but it is plain that for many people whose behavior concerns us deeply, the consequences they in fact face do not matter much, or enough, or at all.
Recent research from Cincinnati, Ohio provides a case in point. A project aimed at understanding and addressing homicide and violent crime identified some sixty-nine violent groups—drug crews and the like—comprising something less than 1,000 individuals. Nearly 750 of those individuals were identified by name and found in police records. Those individuals, most of them fairly young, averaged more than fourteen prior misdemeanor charges, more than ten misdemeanor convictions, more than seven felony charges, three felony convictions, nearly thirteen charges as juveniles, and nearly nine convictions as juveniles: which is to say, all told, they averaged nearly thirty-five charges and nearly twenty-two convictions apiece. Nearly 57 percent had ten or more misdemeanor charges; nearly a third had ten or more felony charges. This is the very model of failed deterrence. And here, as often, when deterrence fails it matters a great deal: These groups of offenders were associated with nearly three-quarters of the homicides in Cincinnati.32
Here too, evaluations of real-world policies support the failure of deterrence. The seminal Kansas City Preventive Patrol Experiment varied vehicular police patrol in Kansas City and found no difference in crime, fear, or public perception between a standard level of patrol, twice that level, and no patrol at all.33 This is often taken to imply that patrol is not effective, and even that policing is not effective; it means neither, but it can be taken to imply that patrol more or less as it is in fact usually practiced is not effective. An evaluation of police foot patrol in Newark found reductions in residents’ fear but no reduction in serious crime.34 Studies of mandatory sentences for gun crime...

Table of contents

  1. Front Cover
  2. Editors
  3. Deterrence and Crime Prevention
  4. Routledge Studies in Crime and Economics
  5. Title Page
  6. Copyright
  7. Dedication
  8. Contents
  9. List of illustrations
  10. Acknowledgments
  11. Introduction
  12. 1. Does deterrence work?
  13. 2. How, and do, criminals think? The classical deterrence framework and the meaning of rationality
  14. 3. Some implications of the subjectivity of deterrence
  15. 4. Initial reflections: From within the traditional framework
  16. 5. Crime and criminal-justice practice: The context of deterrence
  17. 6. The criminogenic implications of official practice
  18. 7. Reflections II: Amending the deterrence framework
  19. 8. Reframing deterrence
  20. 9. Applications I: Eliminating overt drug markets: The “High Point” strategy
  21. 10. Applications II: A thought experiment: The framework of a deterrence approach to domestic violence
  22. 11. Listening to Lysistrata
  23. Notes
  24. Index