Happiness and the Law
eBook - ePub

Happiness and the Law

  1. English
  2. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  3. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub
Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

Happiness and the law. At first glance, these two concepts seem to have little to do with each other. To some, they may even seem diametrically opposed. Yet one of the things the law strives for is to improve people's quality of life. To do this, it must first predict what will make people happy. Yet happiness research shows that, time and time again, people err in predicting what will make them happy, overestimating the import of money and mistaking the circumstances to which they can and cannot adapt.  Drawing on new research in psychology, neuroscience, and economics, the authors of Happiness and the Law assess how the law affects people's quality of life—and how it can do so in a better way. Taking readers through some of the common questions about and objections to the use of happiness research in law and policy, they consider two areas in depth: criminal punishment and civil lawsuits. More broadly, the book proposes a comprehensive approach to assessing human welfare—well-being analysis—that is a valuable alternative to the strictly economically based cost-benefit analyses currently dominating how we evaluate public policy. The study of happiness is the next step in the evolution from traditional economic analysis of the law to a behavioral approach. Happiness and the Law will serve as the definitive, yet accessible, guide to understanding this new paradigm.

Frequently asked questions

Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Yes, you can access Happiness and the Law by John Bronsteen,Christopher Buccafusco,Jonathan S. Masur in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Law & Law Theory & Practice. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Year
2014
ISBN
9780226195667
Topic
Law
Index
Law
PART I
Analyzing Laws’ Effects on Well-Being
The law should improve people’s quality of life. For it to do that, lawmakers need a way to measure the effect of proposed laws on well-being. In the chapters that follow, we explain how the data from he donic psychology can be used to improve policymaking in this way. Neither these data nor our methodology is a panacea. Our proposal is not addressed to many of the grand questions of political theory or of policymaking, such as how to balance the overall quality of human life against other considerations, such as fairness or concern for nonhuman animals, among other things. We offer only a new way to measure how law affects human well-being.
This project, of course, does not encompass everything that matters in policymaking. But it does represent an attempt to overcome some of the fundamental limitations of recent policy analysis. For the past thirty years, the primary driver of policy analysis has been the attempt to quantify well-being using cost-benefit analysis. If our proposal, well-being analysis, improves upon the way well-being is quantified, then it constitutes a step forward in the way society makes law.
CHAPTER ONE
Measuring Happiness
What is it like to be injured on a job site and lose a limb? What is it like to be unemployed for a period of time, or to be imprisoned? What is it like to live with poor air quality, or to be prevented from engaging in free expression? Being able to answer these questions accurately is essential to the proper functioning of a legal system. If the law fails to do so, it will struggle to provide adequate compensation for injuries, to punish people for their crimes, and to protect people from harm. Moreover, if the tools a legal system uses to provide answers to these questions are unreliable and inconsistent, similar cases may not be treated similarly. Yet despite the centrality of these questions to the law, there have been surprisingly few attempts to answer them in a rigorous and systematic way for use in legal analysis. This shortcoming can probably be blamed on limited data and problematic assumptions. It was simply too difficult to know, in a way that could be tested meaningfully by the best tools of social science, what losing a limb or being sent to prison is like.
That is no longer the case. The rapidly emerging field of hedonic psychology is now supplying valid and reliable data that can help lawmakers and legal scholars answer these (and many more) important questions. It is now possible to estimate fairly accurately how the experience of losing a limb or being imprisoned is going to make most people feel. How? Simply by asking people who are undergoing those experiences. Relying on people’s self-reports of their subjective well-being (SWB), researchers in a number of fields have developed sophisticated and scientifically validated methods for measuring the effects of many circumstances on people’s happiness. Importantly, their discoveries are often highly counter intuitive. For example, research has shown that human beings have an astonishing ability to hedonically adapt to changes in their life circumstances. Many seemingly momentous changes will exert surprisingly little long-term hedonic effect on our lives. Yet, also counterintuitively, some seemingly minor changes may have extended effects on our happiness.
This relates to the second important discovery from hedonic psychology. People are often not very good at predicting what will make them happy. Certainly people accurately predict that hitting a hole-in-one will feel better than being hit by a truck, but people often make systematic errors in their estimates of the magnitude and duration of changes in their lives. Often these “affective forecasting errors” occur because people neglect the effects of hedonic adaptation, causing them to overestimate how happy or unhappy many changes will make them feel.
In this chapter we introduce this research in hedonic psychology. We begin by discussing the techniques used to gather happiness data, and then we report on some of hedonic psychology’s major findings, those that will be useful again and again throughout the book. Finally, we briefly address some of the most common questions and concerns about using happiness data to inform legal analysis.
The Data of Hedonic Psychology
How can we learn what makes people feel good or bad? The primary way is simply to ask them how they feel at various moments during their day and during their life. Happiness is thus principally studied via self-reports: psychologists learn how people feel by recording what they say about their feelings. Then psychologists try to replicate the results by repeating the studies, and they also compare people’s self-reports to other indicia of happiness such as others’ reports and neurological and other physiological indicators. These efforts have been highly successful in validating the self-reports, which is why the field of happiness research has received so much attention in recent years.
Social scientists have been attracted to the idea of measuring human welfare directly for a long time, but until recently they have had difficulty securing valid and reliable data.1 Over the last fifteen years or so, new social science techniques have emerged that enable researchers to study subjective well-being from a variety of different perspectives with a number of different tools.2 These techniques allow the more or less direct measurement of people’s happiness levels, overcoming the problem that had initially driven economists to seek monetary proxies for welfare. Importantly, they enable the measurement of what Daniel Kahneman has termed “experienced utility” (how good people feel) in contrast to the “decision utility” that is typically studied in the tradition of law and economics.3 “Decision utility” measures only whether people get what they want, on the assumption that getting it will make them better off. But because that assumption has been shown to be flawed,4 Kahneman and others have turned toward measuring directly the quality of people’s experience of life. This section will briefly discuss a few of the most promising techniques for collecting such experiential data and their relative strengths and weaknesses.
Experience sampling methods
The best way to figure out how an experience makes a person feel is to ask her about it while she is experiencing it. The “gold standard” of such measures is the experience sampling method (ESM), which uses handheld computers and smartphones to survey people about their experiences.5 Subjects are beeped randomly throughout the day and asked to record what they are doing and how they feel about it. The data that emerge from such studies provide a detailed picture of how people spend their time and how their experiences affect them. The data can also be combined with socio-economic and demographic data via regression analyses for even greater insight (e.g., do the unemployed spend more time in leisure activities than the employed, and do they enjoy them as much?).
Unlike some of the other measures of well-being discussed below, ESM studies do not require people to engage in difficult cognitive processes like remembering and aggregating experiences over large chunks of time. Those processes can cause errors in data collection that ESM seeks to avoid. ESM studies can, however, be expensive and difficult to run, so researchers have sought other methods that produce most of the advantages of ESM but at a lower price. One such technique is the day reconstruction method (DRM) pioneered by Daniel Kahneman and his colleagues. DRM uses daily diary entries about each day’s experiences to reconstruct an account of subjects’ emotional lives. DRM studies correlate strongly with ESM studies and can be run at lower cost.6 Similarly, the Princeton Affect and Time Survey (PATS) asks subjects to report and evaluate their experiences from the previous day.7 It can be distributed via telephone and incorporated into other survey devices, enabling it to reach a larger population.8
Life satisfaction surveys
The oldest method of measuring SWB is the life satisfaction survey. These surveys ask individuals to respond to a question such as, “All things considered, how satisfied with your life are you these days?”9 Respondents answer on a scale that ranges from “not very happy” to “very happy.” Life satisfaction surveys have been included in the U.S. General Social Survey since the 1970s; as a result, we now have substantial quantities of longitudinal data on thousands of individuals. The principal value in such surveys is the ability to correlate SWB data with a variety of other facts about people’s lives. Using multivariate regression analyses that control for different circumstances, researchers are able to estimate the strength of the correlations between SWB and factors such as income, divorce, unemployment, disability, and the death of family members.10 For example, on average, the death of a parent will yield the loss of 0.25 life satisfaction points on a scale of 1 to 7 for a period of time, while the death of a spouse will typically yield the loss of 0.89 points.11
Life satisfaction surveys are relatively inexpensive to administer and can be easily included in a variety of larger survey instruments. Accordingly, they are most valuable as sources of large-scale data about many subjects and of longitudinal data about changes in SWB over time. In longitudinal studies, subjects are tracked over long periods of time so that changes in their well-being can be followed. This is especially valuable in assessing the causal effects of life events (such as marriage, disability, or unemployment) on SWB, because the same individual can be surveyed both before and after the event, eliminating the need to make comparisons between people who might be different in a number of important but unmeasured ways.12 Life satisfaction surveys are less helpful, however, for assessing particularly granular changes in circumstances. More importantly, they rely on global judgments about how people’s lives are going, rather than those individuals’ moment-by-moment hedonic experiences. Because hedonic experiences are often poorly remembered and aggregated, such judgments can be biased because of a person’s momentary mood or the order in which questions are posed, among other errors.13
The quality of the data
The ability to generate data is not the same as the ability to actually measure the thing sought to be measured. Nor is it the ability to measure it well. Data are only useful if they are reliable and valid. Although he donic psychology is a relatively young science, it is already producing data that are trustworthy.
Reliability is an indication of the consistency of a measurement instrument.14 For example, a scale that reported very similar numbers every time the same weight was placed on it would be judged highly reliable. In the context of well-being measures, reliability can be assessed by examining correlations between tests and retests of the same question at separate times, as well as correlations between different questions that ask about similar concepts.15 Meta-analyses of different well-being tools have found high levels of reliability for both life satisfaction and experience sampling methods.16 This is especially true of more advanced multiitem measures.17
The fact that a measure reliably provides consistent data does not mean that it is measuring what you want it to measure.18 The ability to actually measure the thing sought to be measured is called validity.19 Although a full review of the validity of well-being measures is unnecessary here,20 it is worth noting a number of findings that support the conclusion that a person’s well-being can be validly measured by the tools discussed above.
One way of thinking about the validity of happiness measures is to ask how well they are associated with other indicators of well-being. If happiness data are measuring a valid concept, then they should be correlated both with other subjective well-being data and with other, objective well-being indicators. The happiness data score well on both of these fronts. First, despite the rather different techniques used to collect data, the various measures of well-being tend to correlate with one another.21 Overall life satisfaction is correlated both with the amount of positive and negative affect (emotion) that a person feels22 and with her satisfaction with the domains of her life (e.g., family, work, friends).23 As most theories of well-being would predict, the happier a person feels on a moment-by-moment basis, the happier she judges her life to be. In addition, if a person is not happy with areas of his life that we might believe are importan...

Table of contents

  1. Cover
  2. Copyright
  3. Title Page
  4. Dedication
  5. Contents
  6. Acknowledgments
  7. Introduction: What Happiness Has to Do with the Law
  8. Part I. Analyzing Laws’ Effects on Well-Being
  9. Part II. Viewing Two Core Areas of the Law through the Lens of Hedonics
  10. Part III. Well-Being
  11. Conclusion: The Future of Happiness and the Law
  12. Notes
  13. Bibliography
  14. Index