Designing Experimental Research in Archaeology
eBook - ePub

Designing Experimental Research in Archaeology

Examining Technology through Production and Use

Jeffrey R. Ferguson

  1. English
  2. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  3. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Designing Experimental Research in Archaeology

Examining Technology through Production and Use

Jeffrey R. Ferguson

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

Designing Experimental Research in Archaeology is a guide for the design of archaeological experiments for both students and scholars. Experimental archaeology provides a unique opportunity to corroborate conclusions with multiple trials of repeatable experiments and can provide data otherwise unavailable to archaeologists without damaging sites, remains, or artifacts.

Each chapter addresses a particular classification of material culture-ceramics, stone tools, perishable materials, composite hunting technology, butchering practices and bone tools, and experimental zooarchaeology-detailing issues that must be considered in the development of experimental archaeology projects and discussing potential pitfalls. The experiments follow coherent and consistent research designs and procedures and are placed in a theoretical context, and contributors outline methods that will serve as a guide in future experiments. This degree of standardization is uncommon in traditional archaeological research but is essential to experimental archaeology.

The field has long been in need of a guide that focuses on methodology and design. This book fills that need not only for undergraduate and graduate students but for any archaeologist looking to begin an experimental research project.

Frequently asked questions

Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Yes, you can access Designing Experimental Research in Archaeology by Jeffrey R. Ferguson in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Social Sciences & Sociology. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Year
2010
ISBN
9781607320234

[ CHAPTER ONE ]

Introduction

Erik J. Marsh and Jeffrey R. Ferguson

SOURCES OF ANALOGIES

Much of what archaeologists understand about variation in material culture and its behavioral correlates is derived from studies that create analogies with past behavior using modern material procurement, manufacture, use, reuse, and discard (Mathieu 2002; Stone and Planel 1999). These analogies generally describe two divergent methodologies that share a theoretical base: ethnoarchaeology and experimental archaeology. This volume focuses on experimental archaeology, “the fabrication of materials, behaviors, or both in order to observe one or more processes involved in the production, use, discard, deterioration, or recovery of material culture” (Skibo 1992a:18). This methodology offers a high degree of control of variables and explores specific research questions not usually accessible in ethnoarchaeological studies.
While ethnoarchaeological research can observe the production of material culture in a wider context, it can also be prone to the erroneous assumption that “all technological knowledge is explicit and can be elicited from any practitioner of the technology” (Schiffer and Skibo 1987:596). Informants in ethnoarchaeological projects may not want or be able to clarify the production and use of their own materials, whereas experimental archaeology can directly address questions such as how different tempers affect thermal performance characteristics of ceramic vessels (Beck, Harry, this volume; Schiffer and Skibo 1987). Ethnoarchaeological research has great utility for examining material culture in its social context, but experimental archaeology is preferred for isolating the effects and relationships of small sets of related variables (such as how stone flake length relates to platform thickness and angle).
The common theoretical foundation of ethnoarchaeology and experimental archaeology allows researchers to tack between complementary analogies generated by the distinct methodologies (Skibo 1992a). While this volume focuses on experimental archaeology, its authors relate their projects to inferences from eth-noarchaeology; their results provide hypotheses to be tested in less controlled eth-noarchaeological settings, where the effects of unanticipated or untested variables and factors can be observed. Future research stands to profit from continued close integration of these two methodologies in comprehensive research programs.

EXPERIMENTAL ARCHAEOLOGY

Izumi Shimada (2005:608) has lamented that little has changed in the decades since Ruth Tringham (1978:171) argued that “experiments in archaeology have for the most part been justifiably ignored because of (1) their lack of a strong theoretical base and a resulting lack of general applicability in testing archaeological hypotheses . . . and (2) their lack of rigor and attention to scientific experimental procedure in design, execution, recording, and analysis.” While experimental archaeology may not always “furnish a foundation for explaining technological variation and change” (Schiffer et al. 1994:198), it is having increasingly greater influence on archaeological inference, as is its close cousin, ethnoarchaeology (Skibo 1992b; Stark 2003). This trend has developed largely through the efforts of Michael Schiffer, James Skibo, and colleagues (1994), who have implemented integrated experimental programs based soundly in scientific methods and directed expressly at archaeological issues.
The chapters in this volume build on the foundation established by Schiffer and colleagues (1994) to contribute more directly to archaeological inference through controlled experimentation. To accomplish this, experiments are theoretically con-textualized and conducted with rigorous attention to research design and procedure. These foundational principles allow the modern analogies generated by experimental archaeology to clarify past behaviors and practices. The projects presented here are “nested within families of related principles” (Schiffer et al. 1994:198) as well as within suites of related experiments, as part of long-term, multifaceted experimental research programs. Such programs explore diverse behavioral patterns and their relationships in the complex, long-term processes of site formation, moving research well beyond Tringham’s (1978) criticism.
Future research in experimental archaeology may be able to take cues from recent developments in ceramic ethnoarchaeology. While the chapters in this, volume generally follow the “behavioral archaeology” approach developed in the 1980s by Schiffer and colleagues (1994), recent research in ceramic ethnoarchaeology shows a much broader range of theoretical approaches (Stark 2003:199) that may foreshadow new avenues for experimental archaeology. Another similarity may lie in the regional focus of many Americanist ceramic ethnoarchaeologists, who often overlook research from other areas of the world or research not published in English, leading to the fragmentation of ceramic ethnoarchaeology (Stark 2003:215). Within experimental archaeology, it may be prudent to engage current theoretical tensions emerging in ceramic ethnoarchaeology and better integrate research from around the world.

RESEARCH DESIGN

The chapters in this volume follow coherent and consistent research designs and procedures, focused on the goal of contributing to inference, such as developing material expectations for archaeological data (Lubinski and Shaffer, this volume). The authors place their experiments in a theoretical context, making comparisons either to similar experiments conducted by the same research program or to other well-documented experiments. Experiments establish themselves as relevant to archaeological inference through research designs that explicitly address existing theories based on previous research.
Philip Carr and Andrew Bradbury (this volume) suggest that research design can be improved by heeding lessons from other sciences, such as A. Franklin’s (1981) characterization of “good” experiments. For Franklin, three types of good experiments—crucial, corroborative, and new phenomena—relate data to theory. Crucial experiments support an existing explanation or theory over an alternate explanation. Corroborative experiments furnish support for the basic idea of a single theory. New phenomena experiments produce results not expected based on existing explanations or theories, which may lead to the development of new theories. Karen Harry (this volume) follows a similar theoretical approach, arguing that experiments begin with a hypothesis or question that can be made more or less plausible by examining its consequences. Hence, effective research design makes explicit relationships with existing theories, and well-documented results can be directly integrated into subsequent experiments (Kingery 1982).
While archaeologists routinely work with small sample sizes from non-repeat-able excavations, experimental archaeology provides a unique opportunity to corroborate conclusions through multiple trials of repeatable experiments. This methodology is fundamental to any scientific experiment relevant to theory (Carr and Bradbury, Harry, Lubinski and Shaffer, this volume) and can provide data otherwise unavailable to archaeologists. In the excavation of archaeological sites, “[n]o matter how much we dig, we work with impossibly small, unrepresentative samples of data” (Lekson 2008:13). However, data from controlled experiments can be completely recovered and resampled to better assess how well small samples represent a larger population (e.g., Lubinski and Shaffer, this volume). Experiments can test for equifinity, where multiple causes lead to the same effect—a persistent issue in interpretations of archaeological data (Carr and Bradbury, Lubinski and Shaffer, this volume). For example, the damaged edge of a lithic artifact may be the result of diverse combinations of human and non-human influences, which can be sorted out through experimentation (Bamforth, this volume). Multiple trials of controlled experiments that explore alternate causes with a common result can directly contribute to reducing ambiguity in the interpretation of archaeological data.
These advantages of experimental archaeology result from adherence to the scientific method and, as the authors in this volume emphasize, from multiple repetitions of the same experiment that explore alternate possibilities. To address this strategy, the authors outline step-by-step methods specific to their materials that will guide future experiments. This degree of standardization is uncommon in traditional archaeological research, but it is essential to experimental archaeology.

DIVERSE RESEARCH SETTINGS:
GREATER AND LESSER CONTROL OF VARIABLES

Experimental archaeology offers the choice to control some of the manifold variables involved in the use of materials in the past. To clarify the complex interactions of these variables, experimental archaeology has developed sophisticated modern analogies (Mathieu 2002; Stone and Planel 1999). Experimenters can decide how carefully to control variables and tack among highly controlled lab settings, more “natural” field settings (Lubinski and Shaffer, this volume), and, in some cases, ethno-archaeological observations. These complementary methodologies share a common theoretical base (Schiffer 1987; Skibo 1992a, 1992b; Stark 2003; Tringham 1978), although with different research foci and potential. Many effective research programs integrate lab, “natural,” and ethnoarchaeological observations, an approach that has the most potential to provide data that directly impact archaeological inference (e.g., Skibo 1992b).
The research presented in this volume focuses primarily on controlled laboratory experiments, but it also relies on related experiments and research from field and ethnoarchaeological settings. Controlled laboratory experiments are characterized by their replicability and tight control of very few variables, usually in a laboratory setting. Field experiments relax control of variables to more closely replicate possible prehistoric situations, thereby becoming less repeatable and more subject to equifinity (Lubinski and Shaffer, this volume). Different settings are appropriate for different research questions, and the most effective projects use a variety of approaches (Harry, Jolie and McBrinn, this volume).

Greater Control of Variables

Highly controlled experiments examine a narrow range of variables in laboratory settings under replicable conditions and suggest relatively “low-level” principles that may seem far removed from archaeological inference (Skibo 1992a, 1992b). Such experiments often focus on the physical and mechanical properties of materials humans use to make tools, most commonly flaked stone (e.g., Dibble and Pelcin 1995; Pelcin 1997; Speth 1975). These low-level experiments often describe universal properties of common materials and apply to the same archaeological material regardless of cultural setting. Greater control of variables also allows for double-blind tests, which have direct bearing on the interpretation of use-wear on archaeological artifacts (Bamforth, this volume). These experiments isolate few variables, so they must be related to other experiments and archaeological observations to have wider relevance.
While highly controlled experiments can be conducted in laboratory settings, projects aimed at learning how real people used real tools to accomplish real tasks in the past should “replicate realistic use contexts to the extent possible” (Bamforth, this volume; see also Harry, this volume), taking into consideration the necessary control of variables appropriate for the specific research question. In use-wear analysis, Douglas Bamforth (this volume) recommends incorporating both field settings for making and using tools and laboratory settings for analyzing use-wear, similar to Harry’s (this volume) approach in selecting and gathering clays to make and test clay tiles in a laboratory.
In another example described by Harry (this volume), controlled laboratory experiments suggested that ceramic vessels with interior surface treatments boiled water more efficiently than those without untreated interiors (Schiffer 1990; Skibo 1992b). However, the same experiment repeated in more “natural” field settings showed that cooking food in vessels without interior surface treatments quickly clogged their pores. After food had been cooked in the vessels, they became as efficient at boiling water as vessels with interior surface treatments (Pierce 1999). Hence, multiple related experiments in different settings have the best potential for understanding past and present cooking practices.
In almost all experiments, the most difficult variable for which to control is the human user. If archaeologists conduct an experiment, they can more closely control the process to fit their ends. However, experimenters are often a poor proxy for those they seek to understand—people who were likely from a different time and culture, who were more skilled at making and using their material culture than are those conducting the experiment, and who have very different goals (Adams, this volume). To address this issue, Raymond Mauldin’s (1993) experiment with ground stone was conducted by a Bolivian woman who was more familiar with, and more expert in, the use of the tools (Adams, this volume). Flintknapping experiments have consistently documented significant differences between amateur and expert knappers (Bamforth and Finlay 2008), and similar differences are evident in atlatl throwers’ distance and accuracy. John Whittaker (this volume) uses data from recreational atlatl competitions as a general guide to the distance and accuracy prehistoric experts may have achieved with the weapon. These examples show how understanding the range of variation in selected human-related variables may help produce a stronger analogy with past behavior, especially when combined with more controlled laboratory analysis.

Lesser Control of Variables

Some ambitious experimental research programs have relaxed control of variables to observe large-scale aggregate effects. Such programs are difficult to repeat but are well suited for generating hypotheses and conducting related controlled experiments. Research conducted since 1972 at Butser Farm, England, has developed a setting analogous to an Iron Age farm. Within this setting, individual controlled experiments have been conducted using agriculture techniques and technology, such as crop rotation, types of manuring, use of livestock to work the fields, soil types, and arable weeds; other experiments have focused on grain storage, construction of buildings, earthworks, metallurgy, and kiln technology (Reynolds 1999a). A similar project at Lejre, Denmark, founded in 1967, builds on earlier Dutch experiments (see Steensberg 1979) and includes experiments focused on tools and construction techniques (Rasmussen and Grþnnow 1999). Axel Steensberg’s experiments in swidden agriculture using imitations of Neolithic tools, conducted in the 1950s, also incorporated large numbers of variables. As in other projects with many uncontrolled variables, these experiments reached few conclusions but did generate hypotheses and ideas to be tested in more controlled settings (Steensberg 1979).
Complex experiments with minimal control over variables are prone to an array of complications. In a long-term project similar to Butser Farm, the Pamunkey Project developed a rough analogy to a Middle Woodland period (ca. AD 1000) settlement in the southeastern United States. This scientifically conceived project produced and analyzed over 700 tools made of various materials, but it ended prematurely as a result of logistical problems, including disputes with the landowner (Callahan 1976). In a much smaller project, Nick Barton and C. Bergman (1982) attempted to compare the spatial dispersal of ancient and experimental lithic scatters in a sand matrix. However, unexpected bioturbation quickly “destroyed” the lithic scatters.

COMPREHENSIVE RESEARCH PROGRAMS

The authors in this volume unanimously call for comprehensive research programs, in contrast to isolated experiments—referred to variously as archaeological, exploratory, orientational, or imaginative experiments (Amick, Mauldin, and Binford 1989; Ascher 1961; Malina 1983; Schiffer et al. 1994). The relatively low cost and ease of conducting “weekend” experiments (Schiffer et al. 1994) make it common for ...

Table of contents

  1. Cover Page
  2. Title Page
  3. Copyright Page
  4. Contents
  5. List of Contributors
  6. List of Figures
  7. List of Tables
  8. Preface
  9. 1. Introduction
  10. 2. Understanding Ceramic Manufacturing Technology: The Role of Experimental Archaeology
  11. 3. Ceramic Vessel Use and Use Alteration: Insights from Experimental Archaeology
  12. 4. Flake Debris and Flintknapping Experimentation
  13. 5. Conducting Experimental Research as a Basis for Microwear Analysis
  14. 6. Experimental Heat Alteration of Lithic Raw Materials
  15. 7. Understanding Grinding Technology through Experimentation
  16. 8. Retrieving the Perishable Past: Experimentation in Fiber Artifact Studies
  17. 9. Weapon Trials: The Atlatl and Experiments in Hunting Technology
  18. 10. Replicating Bone Tools and Other Fauno Technologies
  19. 11. Experimental Zooarchaeology: Research Direction and Methods
  20. Index