Movement in Renaissance Literature
eBook - ePub

Movement in Renaissance Literature

Exploring Kinesic Intelligence

  1. English
  2. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  3. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Movement in Renaissance Literature

Exploring Kinesic Intelligence

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

This book investigates how writers and readers of Renaissance literature deployed 'kinesic intelligence', a combination of pre-reflective bodily response and reflective interpretation. Through analyses of authors including Petrarch, Rabelais, and Shakespeare, the book explores how embodied cognition, historical context, and literary style interact to generate and shape responses to texts. It suggests that what was reborn in the Renaissance was partly a critical sense of the capacities and complexities of bodily movement. The linguistic ingenuity of humanism set bodies in motion in complex and paradoxical ways. Writers engaged anew with the embodied grounding of language, prompting readers to deploy sensorimotor attunement. Actors shaped their bodies according to kinesic intelligence molded by theatrical experience and skill, provoking audiences to respond to their most subtle movements. An approach grounded in kinesic intelligence enables us to re-examine metaphor, rhetoric, ethics, gender, and violence. The book will appeal to scholars and students of English, French, and Italian Renaissance literature and to researchers in the cognitive humanities, cognitive sciences, and theatre studies.

Frequently asked questions

Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Yes, you can access Movement in Renaissance Literature by Kathryn Banks, Timothy Chesters, Kathryn Banks,Timothy Chesters in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Literature & Modern Literary Criticism. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Year
2017
ISBN
9783319692005
Š The Author(s) 2018
Kathryn Banks and Timothy Chesters (eds.)Movement in Renaissance LiteratureCognitive Studies in Literature and Performancehttps://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-69200-5_1
Begin Abstract

1. Introduction

Kathryn Banks1 and Timothy Chesters2
(1)
Durham University, Durham, UK
(2)
Clare College, Cambridge, UK
Let us roll all our strength and all
Our sweetness up into one ball,
And tear our pleasures with rough strife
Through the iron gates of life.
—Andrew Marvell, The Poems of Andrew Marvell, ed. Nigel Smith (Harlow: Pearson, 2007), 84
We are very grateful to contributors to this volume for their valuable comments on a draft of this Introduction.
End Abstract
When the speaker of Marvell’s poem addresses these (almost) closing words to his “coy mistress,” exactly what has he communicated? A powerful sense of urgency, certainly, in keeping with the general thrust of the poem as sexual invitation. But just what are readers supposed to see? If we look more closely, this “image” appears a perfect instance of what Christopher Ricks calls Marvell’s tendency to transcend the visualizable.1 The rolled-up ball of strength and sweetness in the first couplet is evidently connected to the “tearing through” of the second. But how? Is the ball the thing tearing or the thing torn? “Tear through … the iron gates” is unclear too. “With rough strife” could imply the resistance of a closed grill, the pleasures forcibly dragged between its bars. But the gates might equally be open, with a ball-couple tearing along a path through them. Or, perhaps, we might picture the lovers propelling a ball at speed. This faint suggestion is prolonged in the subsequent and final lines of the poem, where we find “one ball” suddenly scaled up to become “our sun” driven along by the poet and his mistress:
Thus, though we cannot make our sun
Stand still, yet we will make him run. (84)
And yet all these things the lover’s words make visible only obscurely, and possibly not at all.
What does seem certain is that the image engages the body as well as the eye: the action verbs “roll … up” and “tear” prompt a powerful sense of force first gathered then unleashed. Sound and rhythm contribute: in the fricative violence of line 3, and as the pace slows in the spondaic “rough strife” only to rattle through the subsequent line. But acknowledging the centrality of embodiment to the image does not make it any more straightforward. That the poet’s mistress might struggle to make global sense of it, for all its embodied immediacy, may not much matter to him. After all immediacy, and not good sense, is precisely what the poet urges: “let’s do this violent, constraint-defying thing,” he seems to say, “and not worry about the details.” The position of the engaged reader or critic, to whom the details do matter, is a little different, however. He or she likewise has no choice but to be struck by the physical shock of Marvell’s lines. But this does not mean that the full potential import of the image is experienced with immediacy. What begins as a pre-conscious response is likely to be expanded into a series of more deliberate rehearsals, as we model in more reflective and conscious ways a wide variety of candidate movements to which the verse appears to gesture, such as forcefully pressing through an iron grill or rolling a ball at speed. Thematically speaking, “To His Coy Mistress” pits spontaneity against deliberation, and hopes the first will win. But, cognitively speaking, the poem calls on a mixture of both from careful readers. It calls on what, drawing on work in the cognitive sciences and cognitive humanities, we propose to call their “kinesic intelligence.”
Since the latter decades of the twentieth century, the cognitive revolution has transformed disciplines as diverse as linguistics, artificial intelligence, psychology, neuroscience and evolutionary biology. “Cognitive” should not be understood here as restricted to so-called higher-order ratiocinative modes of thinking. As philosopher Ray Jackendoff puts it, “cognition” means “an organism’s understanding or grasp of the world and its ability to formulate and execute actions in the world.”2 In other words, the cognitive sciences understand mental processing to be geared towards action, and include within the “cognitive” the non-rational, bodily or emotional. Thus the psychologist Raymond Gibbs warns that “we must not assume cognition to be purely internal, symbolic, computational, and disembodied, but seek out the gross and detailed ways that language and thought are inextricably shaped by embodied action.”3 Indeed, as the philosopher Shaun Gallagher puts it, “the broad argument about the importance of embodiment for understanding cognition has already been made in numerous ways, and there is a growing consensus across a variety of disciplines that this basic fact is inescapable.”4
We cite Gibbs, Gallagher and Jackendoff to give our readers a sense of the importance of the embodied paradigm in general rather than to align ourselves with any version of it in particular. Conceptions of embodied cognition take diverse forms, and have contributed to diverse cognitive literary research. Particularly influential in the so-called “first wave” of literary cognitivism was work by George Lakoff, Mark Johnson and Mark Turner, which argued that language derived metaphorically from our corporeal and spatially oriented life.5 More recently, theories of extended mind or distributed cognition (often grouped together as “4E cognition”) have come to the fore.6 Theorists of Extended Mind (EM) understand the cognitive system to include not only the brain but also the body and the world. In common with other second-wave cognitive literary approaches, and our own, Extended Mind presents cognition as a combination of both universal and historical features, emphasizing neural plasticity and the “extension” of human cognition into language and culture. For instance, literary scholars have employed EM to explain how early modern playing companies dealt with the mnemonic burden of performing up to six plays per week, or brought it into dialogue with Renaissance conceptions of cognition and subjectivity as similarly “extended” or “distributed.”7
Our own approach is grounded in the combination of immediacy and complexity, which we identified in our response to Marvell’s “ball.” The cognitive sciences focus on mental processing that is too swift to be fully registered in our awareness. Hence Daniel Kahnemann’s bestselling Thinking, Fast and Slow draws attention to how much of our cognition is extremely rapid. But literary authors and readers, we suggest, engage in a special task, which is to think both fast and slow. They do this by deploying their “kinesic intelligence.”
The term “kinesic intelligence” was first introduced to the critical lexicon by Ellen Spolsky in 1996, borrowed from psychology and neurology.8 As well as shining a spotlight on our cognitive responses to movement in literature and visual art, Spolsky argued that, especially where art and literature are concerned, embodied knowledge “is not especially privileged in being less ambiguous than other kinds of knowledge.”9 Complexity arose, Spolsky suggested, in the form of conflicts or clashes between bodily knowledge and other forms. Thus Spolsky raised important questions about the degree to which kinesic intelligence was direct or intuitive, and how it related to other modes of knowledge.
Then, armed with insights from more recent scientific work, Guillemette Bolens suggested some new answers to Spolsky’s questions.10 A growing body of research into “motor resonance”—or kinesis—indicates that when we observe the action of another, we access our embodied kinaesthetic memory in order to retrieve a simulation of that action, and so our brains respond in a way similar to when we ourselves execute the movement observed (the phenomenon of so-called “mirror neurons”). Furthermore, motor resonance functions not only when we witness movement but also when we imagine seeing movement, or remember seeing movement, or when we look at visual images of movement, or even when we read a verbal description. Bolens mobilized these insights to argue that “kinesic intelligence in literature is the faculty that enables us to produce and use perceptual simulations in order to understand narrated movements or gestures.” Kinesic then, is not a synonym for kinetic: kinesis refers not to moveme...

Table of contents

  1. Cover
  2. Front Matter
  3. 1. Introduction
  4. 2. Chiastic Cognition: Kinesic Intelligence Between the Reflective and the Pre-reflective in Montaigne and Scève
  5. 3. Turning Toward the Beloved (Virgil, Petrarch, Scève)
  6. 4. Scève’s Denominal Verbs
  7. 5. Metaphor, Lexicography, and Rabelais’s Prologue to Gargantua
  8. 6. The Gunpowder Revolution in Literature: Early Modern Wounds in Folengo and Rabelais
  9. 7. The Finger in the Eye: Jacques Duval’s Traité des Hermaphrodits (1612)
  10. 8. Exchanging Hands in Titus Andronicus
  11. 9. “Cabin’d, Cribb’ed, Confin’d”: Images of Thwarted Motion in Macbeth
  12. 10. Shakespeare’s Vital Signs
  13. 11. Kinesic Intelligence on the Early Modern English Stage
  14. 12. How Do Audiences Act?
  15. Back Matter